Discrimination of temporal synchrony in intermodal events by children with autism and children with developmental disabilities without autism.

BACKGROUND This project examined the intermodal perception of temporal synchrony in 16 young children (ages 4 to 6 years) with autism compared to a group of children without impairments matched on adaptive age, and a group of children with other developmental disabilities matched on chronological and adaptive age. METHOD A preferential looking paradigm was used, where participants viewed non-linguistic, simple linguistic or complex linguistic events on two screens displaying identical video tracks, but one offset from the other by 3 seconds, and with the single audio track matched to only one of the displays. RESULTS As predicted, both comparison groups demonstrated significant non-random preferential looking to violations of temporal synchrony with linguistic and non-linguistic stimuli. However, the group with autism showed an impaired, chance level of responding, except when presented with non-linguistic stimuli. CONCLUSIONS Several explanations are offered for this apparently autism-specific, language-specific pattern of responding to temporal synchrony, and potential developmental sequelae are discussed.

[1]  L. Gogate,et al.  Intersensory redundancy facilitates learning of arbitrary relations between vowel sounds and objects in seven-month-old infants. , 1998, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[2]  Temple Grandin,et al.  Thinking in Pictures: And Other Reports From My Life With Autism , 1996 .

[3]  Lorraine E. Bahrick,et al.  Infants' perception of substance and temporal synchrony in multimodal events* , 1983 .

[4]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Localization of grasp representations in humans by PET: 1. Observation versus execution , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  A. Perry,et al.  Psychometric validity and clinical usefulness of the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales and the AAMD Adaptive Behavior Scale for an autistic sample , 1989, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[6]  K. Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  The eyes have it: lexical and syntactic comprehension in a new paradigm , 1987, Journal of Child Language.

[7]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  The bimodal perception of speech in infancy. , 1982, Science.

[8]  R. Golinkoff,et al.  Revealing hidden competencies: a new method for studying language comprehension in children with motor impairments. , 1989, American journal of mental retardation : AJMR.

[9]  C. Q. Bryson Systematic Identification of Perceptual Disabilities in Autistic Children , 1970, Perceptual and motor skills.

[10]  Katherine A. Loveland,et al.  Intermodal perception of affect in persons with autism or Down syndrome , 1995, Development and Psychopathology.

[11]  D. Zabel,et al.  Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed , 1995 .

[12]  Ami Klin,et al.  Young autistic children's listening preferences in regard to speech: A possible characterization of the symptom of social withdrawal , 1991, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[13]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Speech listening specifically modulates the excitability of tongue muscles: a TMS study , 2002, The European journal of neuroscience.

[14]  E. Spelke Perceiving Bimodally Specified Events in Infancy , 1979 .

[15]  C. Q. Bryson Short-term Memory and Cross-modal Information Processing in Autistic Children , 1972 .

[16]  E. Spelke Infants' intermodal perception of events , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[17]  G. Calvert Crossmodal processing in the human brain: insights from functional neuroimaging studies. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[18]  Perceptual aspects of social knowing: Looking and listening in infancy , 1981 .

[19]  John Bielinski,et al.  Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second Edition , 2008 .

[20]  E. Gibson,et al.  Principles of Perceptual Learning and Development , 1973 .

[21]  T. Bachmann,et al.  Cognitive contributions to the perception of spatial and temporal events (Advances in Psychology, Vol. 129) , 1999 .

[22]  David J. Lewkowicz,et al.  Development of intersensory perception in human infants. , 1994 .

[23]  D. Kolko,et al.  Sensory preference and overselective responding in autistic children , 1980, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[24]  F. Volkmar,et al.  Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders , 1987 .

[25]  Brief report: Preferential looking in intermodal perception by children with autism , 1994, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[26]  L. Schreibman,et al.  Selective responding by autistic children to multiple sensory input. , 1971, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[27]  K. Yamazaki [Autistic disorder]. , 2003, Ryoikibetsu shokogun shirizu.

[28]  Lorraine E. Bahrick,et al.  Amodal relations: The basis for intermodal perception and learning in infancy. , 1994 .

[29]  David J. Lewkowicz,et al.  Chapter 16 The development of temporal and spatial intermodal perception , 1999 .

[30]  E. Courchesne,et al.  Face processing occurs outside the fusiform 'face area' in autism: evidence from functional MRI. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  Dana McDaniel,et al.  Methods for assessing children's syntax , 2000 .

[32]  A. Slater,et al.  Intermodal perception at birth: newborn infants' memory for arbitrary auditory–visual pairings , 1997 .

[33]  E. Sponheim Changing criteria of autistic disorders: A comparison of the ICD-10 research criteria and DSM-IV with DSM-III-R, CARS, and ABC , 1996, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[34]  M. Lamb,et al.  Infant social cognition : empirical and theoretical considerations , 1981 .

[35]  J. Haviland,et al.  Intermodal perception of emotional expressions by children with autism , 1996 .

[36]  L. Bahrick,et al.  Infants' Bimodal Perception of Gender , 1991 .

[37]  M. Legerstee Infants use multimodal information to imitate speech sounds , 1990 .

[38]  Deborah G. Garfin,et al.  Validity and reliability of the Childhood Autism Rating Scale with autistic adolescents , 1988, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[39]  B. Morrongiello,et al.  Developmental changes in associations between auditory-visual events , 1998 .

[40]  Brown Gw,et al.  Convergent and discriminant validities of the Scales of Independent Behavior and the revised Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. , 1990 .

[41]  L. Waterhouse,et al.  Neurofunctional mechanisms in autism. , 1996, Psychological review.

[42]  Barbara Dodd,et al.  Lip reading in infants: Attention to speech presented in- and out-of-synchrony , 1979, Cognitive Psychology.