Imitation in infancy: rational or motor resonance?

The present study investigates the contribution of 2 mechanisms to imitation in infancy. The principle of rational action suggests that infants normatively evaluate the efficiency of observed actions. In contrast, it has been proposed that motor resonance (i.e., the mapping of others' actions onto one's own motor repertoire) plays a central role in imitation. This study tested 14-month-old infants (n = 95) in 5 conditions and manipulated the extent to which the observed actions could be matched onto the infants' own motor repertoire as well as whether the observed behavior appeared to be efficient. The results suggest that motor resonance plays a more central role in imitation in infancy than does a rational evaluation of the observed action.

[1]  C. Keysers,et al.  The Observation and Execution of Actions Share Motor and Somatosensory Voxels in all Tested Subjects: Single-Subject Analyses of Unsmoothed fMRI Data , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[2]  J. Habermas,et al.  The Theory of Communicative Action, Vol. 1, Reason and the Rationalization of Society , 1986 .

[3]  D. Kuhn Education for Thinking , 1986, Teachers College Record: The Voice of Scholarship in Education.

[4]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Baby do – Baby see! How action production influences action perception in infants , 2007 .

[5]  A. Woodward,et al.  Do as I do: 7-month-old infants selectively reproduce others' goals. , 2008, Developmental science.

[6]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Rational Imitation in 12-Month-Old Infants , 2006 .

[7]  A. Wohlschlager,et al.  Action generation and action perception in imitation : An instance of the ideomotor principle , 2003 .

[8]  C. Moore The Development of Commonsense Psychology , 2006 .

[9]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[10]  Gisa Aschersleben,et al.  The Development of Rational Imitation in 9- and 12-Month-Old Infants. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[11]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Enculturated chimpanzees imitate rationally. , 2007, Developmental science.

[12]  P. Hauf Infants' perception and production of intentional actions. , 2007, Progress in brain research.

[13]  T. Chartrand,et al.  THE UNBEARABLE AUTOMATICITY OF BEING , 1999 .

[14]  G. Knoblich,et al.  The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[15]  C. Heyes Causes and consequences of imitation , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  T. Chartrand,et al.  The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[17]  Susan S. Jones PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Imitation in Infancy The Development of Mimicry , 2022 .

[18]  B. Sodian,et al.  Do infants apply the principle of rational action to human agents , 2004 .

[19]  Hein T. van Schie,et al.  You'll never crawl alone: Neurophysiological evidence for experience-dependent motor resonance in infancy , 2008, NeuroImage.

[20]  B. Hommel,et al.  Where Do Action Goals Come from? Evidence for Spontaneous Action–Effect Binding in Infants , 2010, Front. Psychology.

[21]  G. Schöner,et al.  Dynamic Field Theory of Movement Preparation , 2022 .

[22]  B. Elsner Infants' imitation of goal-directed actions: the role of movements and action effects. , 2007, Acta psychologica.

[23]  Jessica A. Sommerville,et al.  Pulling out the intentional structure of action: the relation between action processing and action production in infancy , 2005, Cognition.

[24]  W. Prinz Perception and Action Planning , 1997 .

[25]  R. Byrne,et al.  Priming primates: Human and otherwise , 1998, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[26]  D. L. Mumme,et al.  Doing the right thing: infants' selection of actions to imitate from observed event sequences. , 2007, Child development.

[27]  R. Keesing Cultural Anthropology: A Contemporary Perspective , 1997 .

[28]  A. Meltzoff 'Like me': a foundation for social cognition. , 2007, Developmental science.

[29]  I. Rumfitt,et al.  Making it Explicit: Reasoning, Representing, and Discursive Commitment. , 1997 .

[30]  G. Csibra,et al.  Social learning and social cognition: The case for pedagogy , 2006 .

[31]  D. Lewkowicz,et al.  A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. , 2007, Journal of cognitive neuroscience.

[32]  Z. Nadasdy,et al.  Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age , 1995, Cognition.

[33]  I. Király The effect of the model's presence and of negative evidence on infants' selective imitation. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[34]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Rational tool use and tool choice in human infants and great apes. , 2008, Child development.

[35]  S. Hurley,et al.  Rational Animals ? , 2004 .

[36]  A. Meltzoff Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.

[37]  Harold Bekkering,et al.  The role of frequency information and teleological reasoning in infants' and adults' action prediction. , 2011, Developmental psychology.

[38]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Children's coding of human action: cognitive factors influencing imitation in 3-year-olds. , 2000, Developmental science.

[39]  D. Kuhn How do People Know? , 2001, Psychological science.

[40]  M. Shiffrar,et al.  Fitts's Law Holds for Action Perception , 2007, Psychological science.

[41]  H. Théoret,et al.  EEG evidence for the presence of an action observation–execution matching system in children , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[42]  J. Perner Understanding the Representational Mind , 1993 .

[43]  G. Aschersleben,et al.  The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[44]  M. Brass,et al.  Investigating Action Understanding: Inferential Processes versus Action Simulation , 2007, Current Biology.

[45]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Developmental psychology: Rational imitation in preverbal infants , 2002, Nature.

[46]  G. Csibra,et al.  Teleological reasoning in infancy: the naı̈ve theory of rational action , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[47]  W. Prinz,et al.  The understanding of own and others' actions during infancy: "You-like-me" or "Me-like-you" , 2007 .

[48]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[49]  J. Habermas The Theory of Communicative Action: Reason and the Rationalization of Society , 1986 .

[50]  W. Prinz,et al.  Compatibility between Observed and Executed Finger Movements: Comparing Symbolic, Spatial, and Imitative Cues , 2000, Brain and Cognition.

[51]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[52]  W. Prinz,et al.  The imitative mind : development, evolution, and brain bases , 2002 .

[53]  G. Csibra,et al.  'Obsessed with goals': functions and mechanisms of teleological interpretation of actions in humans. , 2007, Acta psychologica.

[54]  G. Humphreys,et al.  The role of motor simulation in action perception: a neuropsychological case study , 2009, Psychological research.

[55]  Marlies E. Vissers,et al.  Bridging the gap between the other and me: the functional role of motor resonance and action effects in infants' imitation. , 2011, Developmental science.

[56]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Altruistic Helping in Human Infants and Young Chimpanzees , 2006, Science.

[57]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. , 1999, Science.

[58]  J. Spencer,et al.  Generality with specificity: the dynamic field theory generalizes across tasks and time scales. , 2008, Developmental science.

[59]  R. Passingham,et al.  Action observation and acquired motor skills: an FMRI study with expert dancers. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[60]  G. Csibra,et al.  Goal attribution without agency cues: the perception of ‘pure reason’ in infancy , 1999, Cognition.

[61]  B. Hommel Action control according to TEC (theory of event coding) , 2009, Psychological research.

[62]  L. Huber,et al.  Selective Imitation in Domestic Dogs , 2007, Current Biology.

[63]  J. Perner,et al.  Counterfactual reasoning: developing a sense of "nearest possible world". , 2010, Child development.

[64]  Y. Paulignan,et al.  An Interference Effect of Observed Biological Movement on Action , 2003, Current Biology.

[65]  Roel M. Willems,et al.  Complementary Systems for Understanding Action Intentions , 2008, Current Biology.

[66]  Justin N Wood,et al.  The Perception of Rational, Goal-Directed Action in Nonhuman Primates , 2007, Science.

[67]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.