Are release phenomena explained by disinhibited mirror neuron circuits? Arnold Pick's remarks on echographia and their relevance for modern cognitive neuroscience

Background: Echographia, an extremely rare syndrome first described by Pick (1900, 1924), is characterised by automatic translation of visual and sometimes also auditory stimuli into writing. Little information is available about surface symptoms, pathological correlates, and functional mechanisms of this syndrome. The authors thank Dr Philippe Paquier and an anonymous reviewer for their helpful comments on an earlier version of this article. Aims: We here review Pick's original case description of echographia and contrast its surface symptoms and neuropathological correlates with more recently described single cases. We then set out to propose a new theoretical framework for the understanding of echographia which is inspired by contemporary research, including experimental studies in monkeys and neurophysiological and brain‐imaging studies in humans. Main Contribution: Arnold Pick described a patient with severe aphasia after large left perisylvian (frontotemporal) infarction who showed a surprising ability to copy written words that were familiar to her, but no foreign words. Pick believed that the elicitation of echographia by written material resulted from a combination of reflexes and voluntary processes. He suggested that circumscribed damage to the left temporal lobe produced the release of this sort of uncontrolled motor behaviour, and in the present article it is suggested that the involvement of the perisylvian language cortex precluded its full expression. Echographia in modern cases was seen to both written and auditory stimuli, coexisted with incessant writing activity and other environmental‐induced behaviours, and was associated with lesions in the medial frontal and temporal lobes. It is argued that echographia and copious writing in these modern cases represent forms of imitation and utilisation behaviours selective for writing skills which result from relative sparing of fronto‐parietal circuits including audiovisual mirror neurons that control writing actions. Conclusions: Pick's original description of echographia represents the earliest example of utilisation behaviour selective for writing skills. This historical case illuminates the current understanding of the so‐called environmental‐induced behaviours by revealing that such abnormal behaviours can be modality specific. Credit is also given to Pick for integrating audiovisual perception and action in the production of writing, for taking into consideration the permissive role of writing acquisition in the emergence of echographia after brain damage, and for recognising that echographia resulted from released activity of spared parts of the damaged brain.

[1]  C. Mateer,et al.  Evidence of utilization behavior in children with ADHD , 2005, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[2]  M. Sams,et al.  Primary auditory cortex activation by visual speech: an fMRI study at 3 T , 2005, Neuroreport.

[3]  V. Braitenberg,et al.  Entwurf einer neurologischen Theorie der Sprache , 1992, Naturwissenschaften.

[4]  Morten L. Kringelbach,et al.  Visual word recognition: the first half second , 2004, NeuroImage.

[5]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[6]  A. Ardila There is not any specific brain area for writing: From cave-paintings to computers , 2004 .

[7]  F. Pulvermüller,et al.  Distributed neuronal networks for encoding category‐specific semantic information: the mismatch negativity to action words , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.

[8]  C. Mateer,et al.  Utilization Behavior: Clinical Manifestations and Neurological Mechanisms , 2001, Neuropsychology Review.

[9]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neural Circuits Involved in the Recognition of Actions Performed by Nonconspecifics: An fMRI Study , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[10]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Spatiotemporal dynamics of neural language processing: an MEG study using minimum-norm current estimates , 2003, NeuroImage.

[11]  G Jobard,et al.  Evaluation of the dual route theory of reading: a metanalysis of 35 neuroimaging studies , 2003, NeuroImage.

[12]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Two different streams form the dorsal visual system: anatomy and functions , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  L. Fogassi,et al.  Audiovisual mirror neurons and action recognition , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  董瑞国,et al.  Aphasia , 2003, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Human Communication Sciences and Disorders.

[16]  M. Jahanshahi,et al.  Changes in corticospinal motor excitability induced by non-motor linguistic tasks , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Hearing Sounds, Understanding Actions: Action Representation in Mirror Neurons , 2002, Science.

[18]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Is human imitation based on a mirror-neurone system? Some behavioural evidence , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  F. Pulvermüller Brain reflections of words and their meaning , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[20]  J C Mazziotta,et al.  Reafferent copies of imitated actions in the right superior temporal cortex , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  M. Wilson,et al.  Perceiving imitatible stimuli: consequences of isomorphism between input and output. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[22]  D. Perrett,et al.  Imitation, mirror neurons and autism , 2001, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[23]  G. Bi,et al.  Synaptic modification by correlated activity: Hebb's postulate revisited. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[24]  The planning and execution of everyday actions. , 2001 .

[25]  T. Yoshimoto,et al.  [Hyperlexia in an adult patient with lesions in the left medial frontal lobe]. , 2000, Rinsho shinkeigaku = Clinical neurology.

[26]  F. Pulvermüller Words in the brain's language , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[27]  F. Toates The interaction of cognitive and stimulus–response processes in the control of behaviour , 1997, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[28]  Tanaka Ungerleider,et al.  Vision and movement mechanisms in the cerebral cortex , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Explaining Facial Imitation: A Theoretical Model. , 1997, Early development & parenting.

[30]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Inferotemporal cortex and object recognition , 1997 .

[31]  P. Paquier,et al.  Increased writing activity in neurological conditions: a review and clinical study. , 1996, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[32]  J. Rothwell,et al.  Speech‐induced changes in corticospinal excitability , 1996, Annals of neurology.

[33]  E. Renzi,et al.  Imitation and utilisation behaviour. , 1996, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[34]  A. Kertesz,et al.  Arnold Pick and German neuropsychiatry in Prague. , 1996, Archives of neurology.

[35]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[36]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[37]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Neurobiological Mechanisms of Language Acquisition , 1994 .

[38]  E. Capaldi,et al.  The organization of behavior. , 1992, Journal of applied behavior analysis.

[39]  P. Eslinger,et al.  “Frontal lobe” utilization behavior associated with paramedian thalamic infarction , 1991, Neurology.

[40]  C. Wallesch Repetitive verbal behaviour: Functional and neurological considerations , 1990 .

[41]  T. Shallice,et al.  The origins of utilization behaviour. , 1989, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[42]  J. Cambier,et al.  [Graphomania. Compulsive graphic activity as a manifestation of fronto-callosal glioma]. , 1988, Revue neurologique.

[43]  D. O. Hebb,et al.  The organization of behavior , 1988 .

[44]  F. Lhermitte,et al.  Human autonomy and the frontal lobes. Part II: Patient behavior in complex and social situations: The “environmental dependency syndrome” , 1986, Annals of neurology.

[45]  B. Pillon,et al.  Human autonomy and the frontal lobes. Part I: Imitation and utilization behavior: A neuropsychological study of 75 patients , 1986, Annals of neurology.

[46]  M. Mesulam Frontal cortex and behavior , 1986, Annals of neurology.

[47]  J. Locke,et al.  Language acquisition: Speech perception and the emergent lexicon: an ethological approach , 1986 .

[48]  R. de Bleser,et al.  GERMAN WORD FORMATION AND APHASIA , 1986 .

[49]  F. Lhermitte 'Utilization behaviour' and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[50]  Jason W. Brown 21 – Hierarchy and Evolution in Neurolinguistics1 , 1982 .

[51]  N. Geschwind,et al.  Writing disturbances in acute confusional states. , 1972, Neuropsychologia.

[52]  E. Stengel A clinical and psychological study of echo-reactions. , 1947, The Journal of mental science.

[53]  A. Pick ON THE PATHOLOGY OF ECHOGRAPHIA , 1924 .

[54]  A. Benton,et al.  On Aphasia , 1874, British medical journal.

[55]  J. Jackson On the nature of the duality of the brain. , 1874 .