An Active-Inference Approach to Second-Person Neuroscience.

Social neuroscience has often been criticized for approaching the investigation of the neural processes that enable social interaction and cognition from a passive, detached, third-person perspective, without involving any real-time social interaction. With the emergence of second-person neuroscience, investigators have uncovered the unique complexity of neural-activation patterns in actual, real-time interaction. Social cognition that occurs during social interaction is fundamentally different from that unfolding during social observation. However, it remains unclear how the neural correlates of social interaction are to be interpreted. Here, we leverage the active-inference framework to shed light on the mechanisms at play during social interaction in second-person neuroscience studies. Specifically, we show how counterfactually rich mutual predictions, real-time bodily adaptation, and policy selection explain activation in components of the default mode, salience, and frontoparietal networks of the brain, as well as in the basal ganglia. We further argue that these processes constitute the crucial neural processes that underwrite bona fide social interaction. By placing the experimental approach of second-person neuroscience on the theoretical foundation of the active-inference framework, we inform the field of social neuroscience about the mechanisms of real-life interactions. We thereby contribute to the theoretical foundations of empirical second-person neuroscience.

[1]  P. Corlett,et al.  Assumed shared belief about conspiracy theories in social networks protects paranoid individuals against distress , 2023, Scientific reports.

[2]  L. Schilbach,et al.  Interpersonal attunement in social interactions: from collective psychophysiology to inter-personalized psychiatry and beyond , 2022, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B.

[3]  Adrianna C. Jenkins,et al.  A Role for Uncertainty in the Neural Distinction Between Social and Nonsocial Thought , 2022, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[4]  Lancelot Da Costa,et al.  On Bayesian mechanics: a physics of and by beliefs , 2022, Interface Focus.

[5]  D. Mantini,et al.  Disconnection from prediction: A systematic review on the role of right temporoparietal junction in aberrant predictive processing , 2022, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[6]  M. Nassar,et al.  The computational challenge of social learning , 2021, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  Tom Froese,et al.  Shrunken Social Brains? A Minimal Model of the Role of Social Interaction in Neural Complexity , 2021, Frontiers in Neurorobotics.

[8]  S. Eickhoff,et al.  Common brain networks underlying human social interactions: Evidence from large-scale neuroimaging meta-analysis , 2021, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[9]  Rongjun Yu,et al.  Neural correlates of recursive thinking during interpersonal strategic interactions , 2021, Human brain mapping.

[10]  Manuel Baltieri,et al.  Embodied skillful performance: where the action is , 2021, Synthese.

[11]  D. Margulies,et al.  Toward a hierarchical model of social cognition: A neuroimaging meta-analysis and integrative review of empathy and theory of mind. , 2020, Psychological bulletin.

[12]  Peter Dayan,et al.  The Anterior Cingulate Cortex Predicts Future States to Mediate Model-Based Action Selection , 2020, Neuron.

[13]  S. Foote,et al.  Locus coeruleus: a new look at the blue spot , 2020, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[14]  T. Fuchs The Circularity of the Embodied Mind , 2020, Frontiers in Psychology.

[15]  Steve W. C. Chang,et al.  Is There a ‘Social’ Brain? Implementations and Algorithms , 2020, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Sophisticated Inference , 2020, Neural Computation.

[17]  Danilo Bzdok,et al.  Dark control: The default mode network as a reinforcement learning agent , 2020, Human brain mapping.

[18]  P. Strick,et al.  Multiple areas of the cerebral cortex influence the stomach , 2020, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[19]  Ilanit Gordon,et al.  In and out of synchrony-Behavioral and physiological dynamics of dyadic interpersonal coordination. , 2020, Psychophysiology.

[20]  Annett Schirmer,et al.  Interactional synchrony: signals, mechanisms and benefits , 2020, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[21]  Rick A Adams,et al.  Searching for an anchor in an unpredictable world: A computational model of obsessive compulsive disorder. , 2020, Psychological review.

[22]  Oliver Alan Kannape,et al.  Breathing is coupled with voluntary action and the cortical readiness potential , 2020, Nature Communications.

[23]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Active inference on discrete state-spaces: A synthesis , 2020, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[24]  M. Andermann,et al.  Estimation of Current and Future Physiological States in Insular Cortex , 2020, Neuron.

[25]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Deeply Felt Affect: The Emergence of Valence in Deep Active Inference , 2019, Neural Computation.

[26]  Andrew W. Corcoran,et al.  From allostatic agents to counterfactual cognisers: active inference, biological regulation, and the origins of cognition , 2019, Biology & Philosophy.

[27]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Bayesian Filtering with Multiple Internal Models: Toward a Theory of Social Intelligence , 2019, Neural Computation.

[28]  Willem Waegeman,et al.  Aleatoric and epistemic uncertainty in machine learning: an introduction to concepts and methods , 2019, Machine Learning.

[29]  A. Ghazanfar,et al.  The Life of Behavior , 2019, Neuron.

[30]  Malte F. Jung,et al.  Finding the neural correlates of collaboration using a three-person fMRI hyperscanning paradigm , 2019, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[31]  Samuel J. Gershman,et al.  Believing in dopamine , 2019, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[32]  Peter Sterling,et al.  Allostasis: A Brain-Centered, Predictive Mode of Physiological Regulation , 2019, Trends in Neurosciences.

[33]  Simone G. Shamay-Tsoory,et al.  Real-Life Neuroscience: An Ecological Approach to Brain and Behavior Research , 2019, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[34]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A tale of two densities: active inference is enactive inference , 2019, Adapt. Behav..

[35]  Arjen Stolk,et al.  Beyond the Isolated Brain: The Promise and Challenge of Interacting Minds , 2019, Neuron.

[36]  A. Böckler,et al.  Social Impairments in Mental Disorders: Recent Developments in Studying the Mechanisms of Interactive Behavior , 2019, Clinical Psychology in Europe.

[37]  J. Rowe,et al.  Apathy Is Associated With Reduced Precision of Prior Beliefs About Action Outcomes , 2019, bioRxiv.

[38]  P. Cisek Resynthesizing behavior through phylogenetic refinement , 2019, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[39]  A. Hamilton,et al.  Adults imitate to send a social signal , 2019, Cognition.

[40]  E. Redcay,et al.  Using second-person neuroscience to elucidate the mechanisms of social interaction , 2019, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[41]  Paul Cisek,et al.  The Urge to Decide and Act: Implications for Brain Function and Dysfunction , 2019, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[42]  Trygve E Bakken,et al.  Transcriptomic evidence that von Economo neurons are regionally specialized extratelencephalic-projecting excitatory neurons , 2019, Nature Communications.

[43]  A. Shenhav,et al.  Resolving uncertainty in a social world , 2019, Nature Human Behaviour.

[44]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  In the Body’s Eye: The computational anatomy of interoceptive inference , 2019, bioRxiv.

[45]  L. Schilbach,et al.  The Interactive Self – A Review on Simulating Social Interactions to Understand the Mechanisms of Social Agency , 2019, i-com.

[46]  Tom Froese,et al.  Embodied Dyadic Interaction Increases Complexity of Neural Dynamics: A Minimal Agent-Based Simulation Model , 2019, Front. Psychol..

[47]  Ivana Konvalinka,et al.  The body talks: Sensorimotor communication and its brain and kinematic signatures. , 2019, Physics of life reviews.

[48]  Magalie Ochs,et al.  Brain activity during reciprocal social interaction investigated using conversational robots as control condition , 2019, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B.

[49]  L. F. Barrett,et al.  The sense of should: A biologically-based framework for modeling social pressure. , 2019, Physics of life reviews.

[50]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The Anatomy of Inference: Generative Models and Brain Structure , 2018, Front. Comput. Neurosci..

[51]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Free-energy minimization in joint agent-environment systems: A niche construction perspective , 2018, Journal of theoretical biology.

[52]  S. Gallagher,et al.  The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition , 2018 .

[53]  L. Schilbach,et al.  ‘I Interact Therefore I Am’: The Self as a Historical Product of Dialectical Attunement , 2018, Topoi.

[54]  E. Redcay,et al.  Social interaction recruits mentalizing and reward systems in middle childhood , 2018, Human brain mapping.

[55]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Locus Coeruleus tracking of prediction errors optimises cognitive flexibility: An Active Inference model , 2018, bioRxiv.

[56]  S. Gershman,et al.  The Medial Prefrontal Cortex Shapes Dopamine Reward Prediction Errors under State Uncertainty , 2018, Neuron.

[57]  Elizabeth Redcay,et al.  Let's chat: developmental neural bases of social motivation during real-time peer interaction. , 2018, Developmental science.

[58]  Karl J. Friston Am I Self-Conscious? (Or Does Self-Organization Entail Self-Consciousness?) , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[59]  Léa A S Chauvigné,et al.  Taking two to tango: fMRI analysis of improvised joint action with physical contact , 2018, PloS one.

[60]  Justin S. Feinstein,et al.  Interoception and Mental Health: A Roadmap , 2017, Biological psychiatry. Cognitive neuroscience and neuroimaging.

[61]  N. Wenderoth,et al.  Beyond Autism: Introducing the Dialectical Misattunement Hypothesis and a Bayesian Account of Intersubjectivity , 2017, Psychopathology.

[62]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Uncertainty, epistemics and active inference , 2017, Journal of The Royal Society Interface.

[63]  D. Schacter,et al.  Episodic future thinking: mechanisms and functions , 2017, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[64]  J. Brock,et al.  Human agency beliefs influence behaviour during virtual social interactions , 2017, PeerJ.

[65]  Lilian A. E. Weber,et al.  Computational Psychosomatics and Computational Psychiatry: Toward a Joint Framework for Differential Diagnosis , 2017, Biological Psychiatry.

[66]  John P O'Doherty,et al.  A causal account of the brain network computations underlying strategic social behavior , 2017, Nature Neuroscience.

[67]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Embodiment and Schizophrenia: A Review of Implications and Applications. , 2017, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[68]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Higher order intentionality tasks are cognitively more demanding , 2017, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[69]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The Depressed Brain: An Evolutionary Systems Theory , 2017, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[70]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Answering Schrödinger's question: A free-energy formulation , 2017, Physics of life reviews.

[71]  L. Schilbach,et al.  Observing and participating in social interactions: Action perception and action control across the autistic spectrum , 2017, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[72]  M. Tsakiris,et al.  Mentalizing homeostasis: The social origins of interoceptive inference , 2017 .

[73]  Leonhard Schilbach,et al.  Beyond one Bayesian brain: Modeling intra- and inter-personal processes during social interaction: Commentary on “Mentalizing homeostasis: The social origins of interoceptive inference” by Fotopoulou & Tsakiris , 2017 .

[74]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Active interoceptive inference and the emotional brain , 2016, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[75]  M. Tsakiris,et al.  ‘Bodily precision’: a predictive coding account of individual differences in interoceptive accuracy , 2016, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[76]  Stewart Shipp,et al.  Neural Elements for Predictive Coding , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[77]  Lilian A. E. Weber,et al.  Allostatic Self-efficacy: A Metacognitive Theory of Dyshomeostasis-Induced Fatigue and Depression , 2016, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[78]  P. Dayan,et al.  Pharmacological Fingerprints of Contextual Uncertainty , 2016, PLoS biology.

[79]  Erik Rietveld,et al.  The anticipating brain is not a scientist: the free-energy principle from an ecological-enactive perspective , 2016, Synthese.

[80]  A. Hamilton,et al.  Audience effects: what can they tell us about social neuroscience, theory of mind and autism? , 2016, Culture and brain.

[81]  W. Meck,et al.  The Socio-Temporal Brain: Connecting People in Time , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[82]  Zachary C. Irving,et al.  Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: a dynamic framework , 2016, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[83]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Mind-Wandering With and Without Intention , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[84]  M. Ramstead,et al.  Cultural Affordances: Scaffolding Local Worlds Through Shared Intentionality and Regimes of Attention , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[85]  W. Tschacher,et al.  Synchrony in Psychotherapy: A Review and an Integrative Framework for the Therapeutic Alliance , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[86]  Uri Hasson,et al.  Mirroring and beyond: coupled dynamics as a generalized framework for modelling social interactions , 2016, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[87]  A. Böckler,et al.  Are strong empathizers better mentalizers? Evidence for independence and interaction between the routes of social cognition. , 2016, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[88]  T. Penney,et al.  Emotional Voices Distort Time: Behavioral and Neural Correlates , 2016 .

[89]  Daniel C. Richardson,et al.  Breaking the Fourth Wall of Cognitive Science , 2016 .

[90]  Fabian Ramseyer,et al.  Nonverbal Synchrony in Social Interactions of Patients with Schizophrenia Indicates Socio-Communicative Deficits , 2015, PloS one.

[91]  Philipp Kanske,et al.  Dissecting the social brain: Introducing the EmpaToM to reveal distinct neural networks and brain–behavior relations for empathy and Theory of Mind , 2015, NeuroImage.

[92]  Raymond J. Dolan,et al.  Dopamine, reward learning, and active inference , 2015, Front. Comput. Neurosci..

[93]  A. Seth,et al.  The felt presence of other minds: Predictive processing, counterfactual predictions, and mentalising in autism , 2015, Consciousness and Cognition.

[94]  R. Hari,et al.  Centrality of Social Interaction in Human Brain Function , 2015, Neuron.

[95]  Luciano Fadiga,et al.  When gaze opens the channel for communication: Integrative role of IFG and MPFC , 2015, NeuroImage.

[96]  C. Keysers,et al.  Neural pathways of embarrassment and their modulation by social anxiety , 2015, NeuroImage.

[97]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Active inference, communication and hermeneutics , 2015, Cortex.

[98]  Francisco Torreira,et al.  Timing in turn-taking and its implications for processing models of language , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[99]  C. Honey,et al.  Hierarchical process memory: memory as an integral component of information processing , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[100]  Elizabeth J. Kiel,et al.  Current Themes in Understanding Children's Emotion Regulation as Developing from within the Parent-Child Relationship. , 2015, Current opinion in psychology.

[101]  W. K. Simmons,et al.  Interoceptive predictions in the brain , 2015, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[102]  Johanne S. Bjørndahl,et al.  A heart for interaction: Shared physiological dynamics and behavioral coordination in a collective, creative construction task. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[103]  Kobin H. Kendrick The intersection of turn-taking and repair: the timing of other-initiations of repair in conversation , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[104]  Timothy B. Smith,et al.  Loneliness and Social Isolation as Risk Factors for Mortality , 2015, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[105]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Active inference and epistemic value , 2015, Cognitive neuroscience.

[106]  Thomas T. Hills,et al.  Exploration versus exploitation in space, mind, and society , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[107]  Susanne Fuchs,et al.  Take a breath and take the turn: how breathing meets turns in spontaneous dialogue , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[108]  Jean Daunizeau,et al.  The Social Bayesian Brain: Does Mentalizing Make a Difference When We Learn? , 2014, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[109]  L. Uddin Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction , 2014, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[110]  Raymond J. Dolan,et al.  The anatomy of choice: dopamine and decision-making , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[111]  Minoru Asada,et al.  Different impressions of other agents obtained through social interaction uniquely modulate dorsal and ventral pathway activities in the social human brain , 2014, Cortex.

[112]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The Dopaminergic Midbrain Encodes the Expected Certainty about Desired Outcomes , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[113]  Raymond J. Dolan,et al.  Model averaging, optimal inference, and habit formation , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[114]  C. Tallon-Baudry,et al.  The neural subjective frame: from bodily signals to perceptual consciousness , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[115]  J. Perner,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews Fractionating Theory of Mind: a Meta-analysis of Functional Brain Imaging Studies , 2022 .

[116]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Reflections on agranular architecture: predictive coding in the motor cortex , 2013, Trends in Neurosciences.

[117]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Anterior insular cortex and emotional awareness , 2013, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[118]  Karl J. Friston Life as we know it , 2013, Journal of The Royal Society Interface.

[119]  Peter Bossaerts,et al.  The Neural Representation of Unexpected Uncertainty during Value-Based Decision Making , 2013, Neuron.

[120]  Jon G. Allen,et al.  Exposure to interpersonal trauma, attachment insecurity, and depression severity. , 2013, Journal of affective disorders.

[121]  G. Glover,et al.  The Medial Prefrontal Cortex and the Emergence of Self-Conscious Emotion in Adolescence , 2013, Psychological science.

[122]  P. König,et al.  Where's the action? The pragmatic turn in cognitive science , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[123]  Karl J. Friston The fantastic organ , 2013, Brain.

[124]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Canonical Microcircuits for Predictive Coding , 2012, Neuron.

[125]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Predictions not commands: active inference in the motor system , 2012, Brain Structure and Function.

[126]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A Free Energy Principle for Biological Systems. , 2012, Entropy.

[127]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Planning as inference , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[128]  A. Roepstorff,et al.  The two-brain approach: how can mutually interacting brains teach us something about social interaction? , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[129]  E. D. Paolo,et al.  The interactive brain hypothesis , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[130]  Simon B. Eickhoff,et al.  Games people play—toward an enactive view of cooperation in social neuroscience , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[131]  Evan F. Risko,et al.  Social attention with real versus reel stimuli: toward an empirical approach to concerns about ecological validity , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[132]  P. Sterling Allostasis: A model of predictive regulation , 2012, Physiology & Behavior.

[133]  K. Vogeley,et al.  Parsing the neural correlates of moral cognition: ALE meta-analysis on morality, theory of mind, and empathy , 2012, Brain Structure and Function.

[134]  David E Vaillancourt,et al.  Segregated and overlapping neural circuits exist for the production of static and dynamic precision grip force , 2011, Human brain mapping.

[135]  Penelope A. Lewis,et al.  Ventromedial prefrontal volume predicts understanding of others and social network size , 2011, NeuroImage.

[136]  Soyoung Q. Park,et al.  Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences the Von Economo Neurons in the Frontoinsular and Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2022 .

[137]  Kaitlin E W Laidlaw,et al.  Potential social interactions are important to social attention , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[138]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Action understanding and active inference , 2011, Biological Cybernetics.

[139]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Orbital prefrontal cortex volume correlates with social cognitive competence , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[140]  E. D. Paolo,et al.  Can social interaction constitute social cognition? , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[141]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Attention, Uncertainty, and Free-Energy , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[142]  L. Hawkley,et al.  Loneliness Matters: A Theoretical and Empirical Review of Consequences and Mechanisms , 2010, Annals of behavioral medicine : a publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine.

[143]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.

[144]  U. Castiello,et al.  Toward You , 2010 .

[145]  L. Schilbach A second-person approach to other minds , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[146]  Louise C Hawkley,et al.  Perceived social isolation makes me sad: 5-year cross-lagged analyses of loneliness and depressive symptomatology in the Chicago Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study. , 2010, Psychology and aging.

[147]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  Live face-to-face interaction during fMRI: A new tool for social cognitive neuroscience , 2010, NeuroImage.

[148]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Action and behavior: a free-energy formulation , 2010, Biological Cybernetics.

[149]  Christian Keysers,et al.  Acting together in and beyond the mirror neuron system , 2009, NeuroImage.

[150]  R. Nagel,et al.  Neural correlates of depth of strategic reasoning in medial prefrontal cortex , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[151]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Predictive coding under the free-energy principle , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[152]  R. Hari,et al.  Brain basis of human social interaction: from concepts to brain imaging. , 2009, Physiological reviews.

[153]  A. G. Feldman New insights into action–perception coupling , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[154]  S. Krach,et al.  Are women better mindreaders? Sex differences in neural correlates of mentalizing detected with functional MRI , 2009, BMC Neuroscience.

[155]  Karl J. Friston Hierarchical Models in the Brain , 2008, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[156]  Jim M. Monti,et al.  Neural repetition suppression reflects fulfilled perceptual expectations , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[157]  S. Lyubomirsky,et al.  Rethinking Rumination , 2008, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[158]  F. Binkofski,et al.  Can Machines Think? Interaction and Perspective Taking with Robots Investigated via fMRI , 2008, PloS one.

[159]  Peter Bossaerts,et al.  Neural correlates of mentalizing-related computations during strategic interactions in humans , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[160]  Jonathan Evans Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[161]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Learning the value of information in an uncertain world , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[162]  Angela J. Yu,et al.  Phasic norepinephrine: A neural interrupt signal for unexpected events , 2006, Network.

[163]  J. Allman,et al.  Intuition and autism: a possible role for Von Economo neurons , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[164]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A theory of cortical responses , 2005, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[165]  Vasudevi Reddy,et al.  Participants Don’t Need Theories , 2004 .

[166]  P. Hof,et al.  Stereologic characterization and spatial distribution patterns of Betz cells in the human primary motor cortex. , 2003, The anatomical record. Part A, Discoveries in molecular, cellular, and evolutionary biology.

[167]  Paul Schrater,et al.  Shape perception reduces activity in human primary visual cortex , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[168]  Miguel A. Costa-Gomes,et al.  Cognition and Behavior in Normal-Form Games: An Experimental Study , 1998 .

[169]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception , 1983, Cognition.

[170]  J. Wertsch From Social Interaction to Higher Psychological Processes. A Clarification and Application of Vygotsky’s Theory , 1979 .

[171]  S. Gallagher,et al.  Getting interaction theory (IT) together: integrating developmental, phenomenological, enactive, and dynamical approaches to social interaction , 2012 .

[172]  E. N. Sokolov Higher nervous functions; the orienting reflex. , 1963, Annual review of physiology.