Interception of targets using brief directional cues

There are time delays in visuomanual and oculomotor pathways, and some of these time delays may be due to the finite time required to process visual motion signals and to extract accurate information about the speed and direction of the motion. The present experiments were designed to ascertain the time required to obtain a reliable estimate of the direction of target motion. Subjects were asked to indicate the final direction of a moving target, which abruptly changed direction and shortly thereafter disappeared, by pointing to its expected emergence at the boundary of an occlusion. Subjects made small but consistent errors that overestimated the target’s change in direction. These errors depended little on the amount of time the target was visible (ranging from 50 to 400 ms) after it changed direction. Pointing direction was strongly correlated with gaze, which was dominated by a saccade initiated shortly after the target changed direction. The pointing errors were explained by the fact that the saccade always intercepted the (occluded) target, but then continued in the same direction toward the boundary of the occlusion. The analysis reveals that target direction was estimated accurately even at the shortest viewing time.

[1]  E. C. Poulton,et al.  Tracking skill and manual control , 1974 .

[2]  J. Findlay Global visual processing for saccadic eye movements , 1982, Vision Research.

[3]  L F Dell'Osso,et al.  Saccades to remembered targets: the effects of smooth pursuit and illusory stimulus motion. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  D. Sparks,et al.  Population coding of the direction, amplitude, and velocity of saccadic eye movements by neurons in the superior colliculus. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[5]  C. Rashbass,et al.  The relationship between saccadic and smooth tracking eye movements , 1961, The Journal of physiology.

[6]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Computational models of cortical visual processing. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[7]  D L Sparks,et al.  Translation of sensory signals into commands for control of saccadic eye movements: role of primate superior colliculus. , 1986, Physiological reviews.

[8]  L E Mays,et al.  Saccades are spatially, not retinocentrically, coded. , 1980, Science.

[9]  S. Lisberger,et al.  Properties of visual inputs that initiate horizontal smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[10]  D. Robinson Eye movements evoked by collicular stimulation in the alert monkey. , 1972, Vision research.

[11]  D. Sparks The brainstem control of saccadic eye movements , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[12]  D. Sparks,et al.  Corollary discharge provides accurate eye position information to the oculomotor system. , 1983, Science.

[13]  E. J. Morris,et al.  Different responses to small visual errors during initiation and maintenance of smooth-pursuit eye movements in monkeys. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  W. Newsome,et al.  Deficits in visual motion processing following ibotenic acid lesions of the middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  W. Newsome,et al.  A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT) , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[16]  Christian Quaia,et al.  Distributed Model of Collicular and Cerebellar Function during Saccades , 2002, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[17]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Temporal properties of visual motion signals for the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  The Duncker illusion and eye-hand coordination. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  John F Soechting,et al.  Interactions between ocular motor and manual responses during two-dimensional tracking. , 2003, Progress in brain research.

[20]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Time constants in the perception of a change in the direction of motion in humans , 2003, Neuroscience Letters.

[21]  David L. Sparks,et al.  Systematic errors for saccades to remembered targets: Evidence for a dissociation between saccade metrics and activity in the superior colliculus , 1994, Vision Research.

[22]  D C Van Essen,et al.  Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  L. Stone,et al.  From following edges to pursuing objects. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  Richard A. Andersen,et al.  Sensorimotor transformation during eye movements to remembered visual targets , 1991, Vision Research.

[25]  E. Adelson,et al.  The analysis of moving visual patterns , 1985 .

[26]  Justin L. Gardner,et al.  Linked Target Selection for Saccadic and Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Manual tracking in two dimensions. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  L. Optican,et al.  Model of the control of saccades by superior colliculus and cerebellum. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  B. Fischer,et al.  Context dependent amplitude modulations of express and regular saccades in man and monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  K C Engel,et al.  Oculomotor tracking in two dimensions. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  G. Orban,et al.  Response latency of macaque area MT/V5 neurons and its relationship to stimulus parameters. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  G H Recanzone,et al.  Shift in smooth pursuit initiation and MT and MST neuronal activity under different stimulus conditions. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  Christian Coëffé,et al.  Reducing the influence of non-target stimuli on saccade accuracy: Predictability and latency effects , 1987, Vision Research.

[34]  David L. Sparks,et al.  Saccades to remembered target locations: an analysis of systematic and variable errors , 1994, Vision Research.

[35]  M. Pavel,et al.  Extrapolation of linear motion , 1992, Vision Research.