Motor System Activation Predicts Goal Imitation in 7-Month-Old Infants
暂无分享,去创建一个
A. Woodward | N. Fox | E. Cannon | P. Ferrari | Courtney A. Filippi | S. Thorpe | Courtney A Filippi | Erin N Cannon | Nathan A Fox | Samuel G Thorpe | Pier F Ferrari | Amanda L Woodward
[1] C. Heyes,et al. Mirror neurons: from origin to function. , 2014, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[2] Joni N. Saby,et al. Infant Brain Responses to Object Weight: Exploring Goal-Directed Actions and Self-Experience. , 2013, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[3] Aapo Hyvärinen,et al. Fast and robust fixed-point algorithms for independent component analysis , 1999, IEEE Trans. Neural Networks.
[4] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[5] Andrew N Meltzoff,et al. Neural correlates of being imitated: An EEG study in preverbal infants , 2012, Social neuroscience.
[6] Roel M. Willems,et al. Complementary Systems for Understanding Action Intentions , 2008, Current Biology.
[7] C. Hofsten,et al. Predictive gaze shifts elicited during observed and performed actions in 10-month-old infants and adults , 2011, Neuropsychologia.
[8] G. Pfurtscheller,et al. Evaluation of event-related desynchronization (ERD) preceding and following voluntary self-paced movement. , 1979, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[9] Graeme S. Halford,et al. Developing Cognitive Competence : New Approaches To Process Modeling , 2015 .
[10] Matthew D. Lieberman,et al. The Busy Social Brain , 2013, Psychological science.
[11] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Goal Representation in Human Anterior Intraparietal Sulcus , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[12] Anne Springer,et al. Repetitive TMS Suggests a Role of the Human Dorsal Premotor Cortex in Action Prediction , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[13] A. Woodward. Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach , 1998, Cognition.
[14] C. Keysers,et al. μ-Suppression during Action Observation and Execution Correlates with BOLD in Dorsal Premotor, Inferior Parietal, and SI Cortices , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[15] G. Csibra. Action mirroring and action understanding: an alternative account , 1993 .
[16] Hein T. van Schie,et al. You'll never crawl alone: Neurophysiological evidence for experience-dependent motor resonance in infancy , 2008, NeuroImage.
[17] Sarah A. Gerson,et al. Mirroring and the development of action understanding , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[18] G. Halford,et al. Variation, Selection, and Cognitive Change , 2015 .
[19] Gregory Hickok,et al. The Myth of Mirror Neurons: The Real Neuroscience of Communication and Cognition , 2014 .
[20] A. Meltzoff,et al. Neural mirroring systems: Exploring the EEG mu rhythm in human infancy , 2011, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.
[21] Jessica A. Sommerville,et al. Pulling out the intentional structure of action: the relation between action processing and action production in infancy , 2005, Cognition.
[22] Sarah A. Gerson,et al. A claw is like my hand: Comparison supports goal analysis in infants , 2012, Cognition.
[23] Heidi Johansen-Berg,et al. Attention to movement modulates activity in sensori-motor areas, including primary motor cortex , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.
[24] Thomas Wolf,et al. Continuous Theta-Burst Stimulation Demonstrates a Causal Role of Premotor Homunculus in Action Understanding , 2014, Psychological science.
[25] Kathryn H. Yoo,et al. The Infant EEG Mu Rhythm: Methodological Considerations and Best Practices. , 2014, Developmental review : DR.
[26] A. Iriki,et al. Mirror neurons through the lens of epigenetics , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[27] Ross E. Vanderwert,et al. Action Experience, More than Observation, Influences Mu Rhythm Desynchronization , 2014, PloS one.
[28] A. Woodward,et al. Do as I do: 7-month-old infants selectively reproduce others' goals. , 2008, Developmental science.
[29] T. Shipley,et al. Effects of brief imitative experience on EEG desynchronization during action observation , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[30] A. Woodward,et al. Relations between infants' emerging reach-grasp competence and event-related desynchronization in EEG. , 2016, Developmental science.
[31] A. Meltzoff. Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.
[32] Mark H. Johnson,et al. Motor System Activation Reveals Infants’ On-Line Prediction of Others’ Goals , 2010, Psychological science.
[33] Neha Mahajan,et al. Seven-Month-Old Infants Selectively Reproduce the Goals of Animate But Not Inanimate Agents. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[34] G Pfurtscheller,et al. Induced Oscillations in the Alpha Band: Functional Meaning , 2003, Epilepsia.
[35] Linda B. Smith,et al. Development as a dynamic system , 1992, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[36] R. Johansson,et al. Action plans used in action observation , 2003, Nature.
[37] Sarah Lloyd-Fox,et al. Goal representation in the infant brain☆ , 2014, NeuroImage.
[38] Mark H. Johnson,et al. Baby steps: investigating the development of perceptual–motor couplings in infancy , 2014, Developmental science.
[39] Yuyan Luo,et al. Toward a Mentalistic Account of Early Psychological Reasoning , 2010, Current directions in psychological science.
[40] H. Wellman,et al. Infant social attention predicts preschool social cognition. , 2004, Developmental science.