The role of co-explanation and self-explanation in learning from design examples of PowerPoint presentation slides

The current study investigated how people learn design principles from examples of PowerPoint presentation slides through self-explanation and co-explanation. This study also explored a strategy to improve the effectiveness of co-explanation by integrating it with a collaborative design activity. Preservice teachers (n = 120) studied the design examples of PowerPoint presentation slides in four research conditions: co-explanation with design, co-explanation, self-explanation, and no prompt (control). Pairs of learners in the co-explanation condition explained fewer strengths and weaknesses of the design examples than nominal pairs in the self-explanation condition. Moreover, co-explanation was not more effective than self-explanation when it came to individual learning outcomes. In contrast, pairs in the co-explanation with design condition were more actively engaged in co-explaining design examples than pairs in the co-explanation condition. This study shows that co-explanation with design is more beneficial for constructing and sharing knowledge of design principles than co-explanation only. This study discussed a trade-off between constructive/interactive learning effects and transactional activity costs in co-explaining design examples.

[1]  Attila Szabó,et al.  Using IT in the undergraduate classroom: should we replace the blackboard with PowerPoint? , 2000, Comput. Educ..

[2]  N. Miyake,et al.  Cognitively active externalization for situated reflection , 2002 .

[3]  Robert A. Bartsch,et al.  Effectiveness of PowerPoint presentations in lectures , 2003, Comput. Educ..

[4]  John R. Anderson,et al.  The role of examples and rules in the acquisition of a cognitive skill. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[5]  M. Lepper,et al.  Intrinsic motivation and the process of learning: Beneficial effects of contextualization, personalization, and choice. , 1996 .

[6]  Michelene T. H. Chi,et al.  Knowledge convergence and collaborative learning , 2007 .

[7]  H. Mandl,et al.  Learning from Worked-Out Examples: The Effects of Example Variability and Elicited Self-Explanations , 1998, Contemporary educational psychology.

[8]  R. Mayer,et al.  Nine Ways to Reduce Cognitive Load in Multimedia Learning , 2003 .

[9]  Alexander Renkl,et al.  Computer-supported example-based learning: When instructional explanations reduce self-explanations , 2006, Comput. Educ..

[10]  Gavriel Salvendy,et al.  Information retention from PowerPointTM and traditional lectures , 2009, Comput. Educ..

[11]  Kurt VanLehn,et al.  Shall We Explain? Augmenting Learning from Intelligent Tutoring Systems and Peer Collaboration , 2008, Intelligent Tutoring Systems.

[12]  F. Fischer,et al.  Knowledge convergence in collaborative learning: Concepts and assessment , 2007 .

[13]  Krista E. DeLeeuw,et al.  A Comparison of Three Measures of Cognitive Load: Evidence for Separable Measures of Intrinsic, Extraneous, and Germane Load , 2008 .

[14]  B. Rittle-Johnson,et al.  Developing Conceptual Understanding and Procedural Skill in Mathematics: An Iterative Process. , 2001 .

[15]  Michelene T. H. Chi,et al.  Observing Tutorial Dialogues Collaboratively: Insights About Human Tutoring Effectiveness From Vicarious Learning , 2008, Cogn. Sci..

[16]  Matthew W. Lewis,et al.  Self-Explonations: How Students Study and Use Examples in Learning to Solve Problems , 1989, Cogn. Sci..

[17]  Elaine H. J. Yew,et al.  The process of problem‐based learning: what works and why , 2011, Medical education.

[18]  H. Schmidt,et al.  The advantages of problem-based curricula. , 1996, Postgraduate medical journal.

[19]  D. Wegner Transactive Memory: A Contemporary Analysis of the Group Mind , 1987 .

[20]  F. Paas,et al.  A Cognitive Load Approach to Collaborative Learning: United Brains for Complex Tasks , 2009 .

[21]  Michelene T. H. Chi,et al.  Active-Constructive-Interactive: A Conceptual Framework for Differentiating Learning Activities , 2009, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[22]  D. Kuhn,et al.  Are self-explanations always beneficial? , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[23]  F. Paas,et al.  Cognitive Architecture and Instructional Design , 1998 .

[24]  T. Gog,et al.  Optimising worked example instruction: Different ways to increase germane cognitive load. , 2006 .

[25]  Rod D. Roscoe,et al.  Tutor learning: the role of explaining and responding to questions , 2008 .

[26]  Sofie M. M. Loyens,et al.  Problem-Based Learning is Compatible with Human Cognitive Architecture: Commentary on Kirschner, Sweller, and Clark (2006) , 2007 .

[27]  K. VanLehn,et al.  Testing the instructional fit hypothesis: the case of self-explanation prompts , 2011 .

[28]  R. Atkinson,et al.  Structuring the Transition From Example Study to Problem Solving in Cognitive Skill Acquisition: A Cognitive Load Perspective , 2003 .

[29]  Alexander Renkl,et al.  Assisting self-explanation prompts are more effective than open prompts when learning with multiple representations , 2009 .

[30]  Herbert A. Simon,et al.  Collaborative Discovery in a Scientific Domain , 1997, Cogn. Sci..

[31]  Wellesley R. Foshay,et al.  Writing Training Materials That Work: How to Train Anyone to Do Anything , 2003 .

[32]  S. Schworm,et al.  Learning argumentation skills through the use of prompts for self-explaining examples. , 2007 .

[33]  F. Paas,et al.  Differential effects of problem-solving demands on individual and collaborative learning outcomes. , 2011 .

[34]  Scotty D. Craig,et al.  Improving Classroom Learning by Collaboratively Observing Human Tutoring Videos while Problem Solving , 2009 .

[35]  Semra Sungur,et al.  Effects of Problem-Based Learning and Traditional Instruction on Self-Regulated Learning , 2006 .

[36]  B. Rittle-Johnson,et al.  Promoting transfer: effects of self-explanation and direct instruction. , 2006, Child development.

[37]  Fred Paas,et al.  Effects of studying sequences of process-oriented and product-oriented worked examples on troubleshooting transfer efficiency , 2008 .

[38]  Daniel L. Schwartz,et al.  The Emergence of Abstract Representations in Dyad Problem Solving , 1995 .

[39]  Brigid Barron When Smart Groups Fail , 2003 .

[40]  David W. Johnson,et al.  An Educational Psychology Success Story: Social Interdependence Theory and Cooperative Learning , 2009 .

[41]  Yair Neuman,et al.  Two Wrongs May Make a Right ... If They Argue Together! , 2000 .

[42]  S. Derry,et al.  Learning from Examples: Instructional Principles from the Worked Examples Research , 2000 .

[43]  Michael Alley,et al.  The Craft of Scientific Presentations: Critical Steps to Succeed and Critical Errors to Avoid , 1987 .

[44]  F. Fischer,et al.  Epistemic and social scripts in computer–supported collaborative learning , 2005 .

[45]  G. Stasser,et al.  Expert Roles and Information Exchange during Discussion: The Importance of Knowing Who Knows What , 1995 .

[46]  H. Schmidt,et al.  Constructivist, Problem-Based Learning Does Work: A Meta-Analysis of Curricular Comparisons Involving a Single Medical School , 2009 .

[47]  Scotty D. Craig,et al.  Constructing Knowledge from Dialog in an Intelligent Tutoring System: Interactive Learning, Vicarious Learning, and Pedagogical Agents , 2004 .

[48]  J. Sweller Element Interactivity and Intrinsic, Extraneous, and Germane Cognitive Load , 2010 .

[49]  Friedrich W. Hesse,et al.  Fostering sharing of unshared knowledge by having access to the collaborators' meta-knowledge structures , 2011, Comput. Hum. Behav..