Young children know that trying is not pretending: a test of the "behaving-as-if" construal of children's early concept of pretense.

In 3 studies, young children were tested for their understanding of pretend actions. In Studies 1 and 2, pairs of superficially similar behaviors were presented to 26- and 36-month-old children in an imitation game. In one case the behavior was marked as trying (signs of effort), and in the other case as pretending (signs of playfulness). Three-year-olds, and to some degree 2-year-olds, performed the real action themselves (or tried to really perform it) after the trying model, whereas after the pretense model, they only pretended. Study 3 ruled out a simple mimicking explanation by showing that children not only imitated differentially but responded differentially with appropriate productive pretending to pretense models and with appropriate productive tool use to trying models. The findings of the 3 studies demonstrate that by 2 to 3 years of age, children have a concept of pretense as a specific type of intentional activity.

[1]  Stephen C. Want,et al.  How do children ape? Applying concepts from the study of non-human primates to the developmental study of 'imitation' in children , 2002 .

[2]  A. Lillard,et al.  Young children's conceptualization of pretense: action or mental representational state? , 1993, Child development.

[3]  T. German,et al.  Immunity to functional fixedness in young children , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[4]  Paul L. Harris,et al.  Young Children's Understanding of Pretense , 1993 .

[5]  R. Joseph,et al.  Intention and knowledge in preschoolers' conception of pretend. , 1998, Child development.

[6]  S. Stich,et al.  A cognitive theory of pretense , 2000, Cognition.

[7]  Philip Smith,et al.  Pretend Play: Is It Metarepresentational? , 1994 .

[8]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Fourteen-through 18-month-old infants di eren-tially imitate intentional and accidental actions , 1998 .

[9]  A. Leslie Pretending and believing: issues in the theory of ToMM , 1994, Cognition.

[10]  Charlie Lewis,et al.  Children's Early Understanding of Mind: Origins and Development , 1994 .

[11]  A. Goldman Theory of Human Action , 1970 .

[12]  Uta Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.

[13]  Catherine S. Tamis-LeMonda,et al.  Child psychology : a handbook of contemporary issues , 2005 .

[14]  A. Lillard,et al.  Body or mind: children's categorizing of pretense. , 1996, Child development.

[15]  H. Wellman The Child's Theory of Mind , 1990 .

[16]  A. Leslie Pretense and representation: The origins of "theory of mind." , 1987 .

[17]  D. Olson,et al.  Developing theories of mind , 1988 .

[18]  Rebekah A. Richert,et al.  Children's understanding of the knowledge prerequisites of drawing and pretending. , 2002, Developmental psychology.

[19]  Stephanie M. Curenton,et al.  Children's Understanding of the Animacy Constraint on Pretense. , 2000 .

[20]  K. Fischer,et al.  A developmental sequence of agent use in late infancy. , 1977 .

[21]  S. Guttenplan Mind and language , 1975 .

[22]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Developmental psychology: Rational imitation in preverbal infants , 2002, Nature.

[23]  Angeline S. Lillard,et al.  Making sense of pretence. , 1994 .

[24]  A. Meltzoff Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.

[25]  J. A. Fodor A theory of the child's theory of mind , 1992, Cognition.

[26]  Harold Bekkering,et al.  Rational imitation of goal-directed actions in 14-month-olds. , 2001 .

[27]  B. Schneuwly Cultural learning is cultural. [A commentary on Tomasello, Krugner and Ratner's "Cultural learning" Peer commentary by B. Schneuwly] , 1993 .

[28]  J. Bruner,et al.  Cultural learning. Author's reply , 1993 .