A Neural Network Model of Ventriloquism Effect and Aftereffect

Presenting simultaneous but spatially discrepant visual and auditory stimuli induces a perceptual translocation of the sound towards the visual input, the ventriloquism effect. General explanation is that vision tends to dominate over audition because of its higher spatial reliability. The underlying neural mechanisms remain unclear. We address this question via a biologically inspired neural network. The model contains two layers of unimodal visual and auditory neurons, with visual neurons having higher spatial resolution than auditory ones. Neurons within each layer communicate via lateral intra-layer synapses; neurons across layers are connected via inter-layer connections. The network accounts for the ventriloquism effect, ascribing it to a positive feedback between the visual and auditory neurons, triggered by residual auditory activity at the position of the visual stimulus. Main results are: i) the less localized stimulus is strongly biased toward the most localized stimulus and not vice versa; ii) amount of the ventriloquism effect changes with visual-auditory spatial disparity; iii) ventriloquism is a robust behavior of the network with respect to parameter value changes. Moreover, the model implements Hebbian rules for potentiation and depression of lateral synapses, to explain ventriloquism aftereffect (that is, the enduring sound shift after exposure to spatially disparate audio-visual stimuli). By adaptively changing the weights of lateral synapses during cross-modal stimulation, the model produces post-adaptive shifts of auditory localization that agree with in-vivo observations. The model demonstrates that two unimodal layers reciprocally interconnected may explain ventriloquism effect and aftereffect, even without the presence of any convergent multimodal area. The proposed study may provide advancement in understanding neural architecture and mechanisms at the basis of visual-auditory integration in the spatial realm.

[1]  Fabrizio Leo,et al.  Independent mechanisms for ventriloquism and multisensory integration as revealed by theta‐burst stimulation , 2010, The European journal of neuroscience.

[2]  A. Georgopoulos,et al.  Cognitive neurophysiology of the motor cortex. , 1993, Science.

[3]  Thomas H. Weisswange,et al.  Bayesian Cue Integration as a Developmental Outcome of Reward Mediated Learning , 2011, PloS one.

[4]  L. Aitkin,et al.  Azimuthal sensitivity of neurons in primary auditory cortex of cats. II. Organization along frequency-band strips. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  G. Recanzone,et al.  The biological basis of audition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[6]  A P Georgopoulos,et al.  On the relations between the direction of two-dimensional arm movements and cell discharge in primate motor cortex , 1982, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[7]  Paul Bertelson,et al.  The aftereffects of ventriloquism: generalization across sound-frequencies. , 2005, Acta psychologica.

[8]  Robert A Jacobs,et al.  Bayesian integration of visual and auditory signals for spatial localization. , 2003, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[9]  E. Rolls,et al.  Neural networks and brain function , 1998 .

[10]  Benjamin A. Rowland,et al.  An Emergent Model of Multisensory Integration in Superior Colliculus Neurons , 2010, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[11]  Jean-Philippe Thiran,et al.  Multisensory interactions within human primary cortices revealed by BOLD dynamics. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[12]  Elisa Magosso,et al.  Integrating Information From Vision and Touch: A Neural Network Modeling Study , 2010, IEEE Transactions on Information Technology in Biomedicine.

[13]  Mauro Ursino,et al.  Neural bases of peri-hand space plasticity through tool-use: Insights from a combined computational–experimental approach , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  Paul Bertelson,et al.  The aftereffects of ventriloquism: Patterns of spatial generalization , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[15]  A. Pouget,et al.  Efficient computation and cue integration with noisy population codes , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  Ilana B. Witten,et al.  Why Seeing Is Believing: Merging Auditory and Visual Worlds , 2005, Neuron.

[17]  A. Ghazanfar,et al.  Is neocortex essentially multisensory? , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Multisensory contributions to low-level, ‘unisensory’ processing , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[19]  G. Recanzone Interactions of auditory and visual stimuli in space and time , 2009, Hearing Research.

[20]  P. Bertelson,et al.  Cross-modal bias and perceptual fusion with auditory-visual spatial discordance , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[21]  Ilja Frissen,et al.  Visual recalibration of auditory spatial perception: two separate neural circuits for perceptual learning , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[22]  P Bertelson,et al.  Auditory-visual interaction and the timing of inputs , 1987, Psychological research.

[23]  Gustavo Deco,et al.  Computational neuroscience of vision , 2002 .

[24]  David R. Wozny,et al.  Computational Characterization of Visually Induced Auditory Spatial Adaptation , 2011, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[25]  Mauro Ursino,et al.  A theoretical study of multisensory integration in the superior colliculus by a neural network model , 2008, Neural Networks.

[26]  H. Sompolinsky,et al.  Theory of orientation tuning in visual cortex. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[27]  Mauro Ursino,et al.  Visuotactile Representation of Peripersonal Space: A Neural Network Study , 2010, Neural Computation.

[28]  G. Orban,et al.  How well do response changes of striate neurons signal differences in orientation: a study in the discriminating monkey , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[29]  M. Wallace,et al.  Visual Localization Ability Influences Cross-Modal Bias , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[30]  Sidney S. Simon,et al.  Merging of the Senses , 2008, Front. Neurosci..

[31]  A. Treves Mean-field analysis of neuronal spike dynamics , 1993 .

[32]  Timothy M. Woods,et al.  Visually Induced Plasticity of Auditory Spatial Perception in Macaques , 2004, Current Biology.

[33]  Kazuyuki Aihara,et al.  Bayesian Inference Explains Perception of Unity and Ventriloquism Aftereffect: Identification of Common Sources of Audiovisual Stimuli , 2007, Neural Computation.

[34]  Haim Sompolinsky,et al.  A Hebbian learning rule mediates asymmetric plasticity in aligning sensory representations. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  Mauro Ursino,et al.  Multisensory integration in the superior colliculus: a neural network model , 2009, Journal of Computational Neuroscience.

[36]  Nadia Bolognini,et al.  Multisensory-Mediated Auditory Localization , 2006, Perception.

[37]  Francis Crick,et al.  The recent excitement about neural networks , 1989, Nature.

[38]  S. Shimojo,et al.  Visual illusion induced by sound. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[39]  A. V. Ooyen Using theoretical models to analyse neural development , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[40]  D. Burr,et al.  Combining visual and auditory information. , 2006, Progress in brain research.

[41]  Paul Bertelson,et al.  The aftereffects of ventriloquism: are they sound-frequency specific? , 2003, Acta psychologica.

[42]  Benjamin A. Rowland,et al.  A computational study of multisensory maturation in the superior colliculus (SC) , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[43]  G. Recanzone,et al.  Frequency and intensity response properties of single neurons in the auditory cortex of the behaving macaque monkey. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  G. Recanzone,et al.  Effects of stimulus azimuth and intensity on the single-neuron activity in the auditory cortex of the alert macaque monkey. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  D. Knill,et al.  The Bayesian brain: the role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.

[46]  M. Wallace,et al.  Unifying multisensory signals across time and space , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Neural Basis of the Ventriloquist Illusion , 2007, Current Biology.

[48]  J. Lewald Rapid adaptation to auditory-visual spatial disparity. , 2002, Learning & memory.

[49]  Wei Ji Ma,et al.  Bayesian inference with probabilistic population codes , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[50]  G. Recanzone,et al.  Temporal and spatial dependency of the ventriloquism effect , 2001, Neuroreport.

[51]  P. Bertelson,et al.  Adaptation to auditory-visual discordance and ventriloquism in semirealistic situations , 1977 .

[52]  Kenneth D. Miller,et al.  The Role of Constraints in Hebbian Learning , 1994, Neural Computation.

[53]  Brigitte Röder,et al.  Tactile recalibration of auditory spatial representations , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  Benjamin A. Rowland,et al.  Hebbian mechanisms help explain development of multisensory integration in the superior colliculus: a neural network model , 2012, Biological Cybernetics.

[55]  G H Recanzone,et al.  Correlation between the activity of single auditory cortical neurons and sound-localization behavior in the macaque monkey. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[56]  D. Burr,et al.  The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration , 2004, Current Biology.

[57]  D. H. Warren,et al.  Immediate perceptual response to intersensory discrepancy. , 1980, Psychological bulletin.

[58]  G. Recanzone Rapidly induced auditory plasticity: the ventriloquism aftereffect. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.