Orientation categories used in guidance of attention in visual search can differ in strength
暂无分享,去创建一个
David Alais | Garry Kong | Erik Van der Burg | D. Alais | E. Van der Burg | Garry Kong | Erik Van der Burg
[1] Bevil R. Conway,et al. Evolution of neural computations: Mantis shrimp and human color decoding , 2014, i-Perception.
[2] David Alais,et al. An investigation of linear separability in visual search for color suggests a role of recognizability. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[3] S P Arun,et al. Does linear separability really matter? Complex visual search is explained by simple search. , 2013, Journal of vision.
[4] D. Hubel,et al. Sequence regularity and geometry of orientation columns in the monkey striate cortex , 1974, The Journal of comparative neurology.
[5] J. Wolfe,et al. Five factors that guide attention in visual search , 2017, Nature Human Behaviour.
[6] Martin Arguin,et al. Conjunction and linear non-separability effects in visual shape encoding , 2000, Vision Research.
[7] Keiji Uchikawa,et al. Color category influences heterogeneous visual search for color. , 2005, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[8] Jonathan I. Flombaum,et al. Why some colors appear more memorable than others: A model combining categories and particulars in color working memory. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[9] S. Appelle. Perception and discrimination as a function of stimulus orientation: the "oblique effect" in man and animals. , 1972, Psychological bulletin.
[10] W. Korte,et al. Über die Gestaltauffassung im indirekten Sehen , 1923 .
[11] Ronald A. Rensink,et al. Sensitivity To Three-Dimensional Orientation in Visual Search , 1990 .
[12] H. BOUMA,et al. Interaction Effects in Parafoveal Letter Recognition , 1970, Nature.
[13] Stefanie I. Becker,et al. Irrelevant singletons in pop-out search: attentional capture or filtering costs? , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[14] J. Wolfe,et al. Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[15] L. Cronbach,et al. Construct validity in psychological tests. , 1955, Psychological bulletin.
[16] BELA JULESZ,et al. Rapid discrimination of visual patterns , 1983, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics.
[17] Jeremy M. Wolfe,et al. Guided Search 4.0: Current Progress With a Model of Visual Search , 2007, Integrated Models of Cognitive Systems.
[18] E. Reingold,et al. Visual search asymmetry: The influence of stimulus familiarity and low-level features , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[19] G W Humphreys,et al. Driving attention with the top down: The relative contribution of target templates to the linear separability effect in the size dimension , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[20] A. Treisman,et al. A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.
[21] P. Malinowski,et al. The effect of familiarity on visual-search performance: Evidence for learned basic features , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[22] Bevil R. Conway,et al. Cerebral Cortex Advance Access published December 28, 2005 Color Architecture in Alert Macaque Cortex , 2022 .
[23] G. F. Cooper,et al. Development of the Brain depends on the Visual Environment , 1970, Nature.
[24] Zijiang J. He,et al. Surfaces versus features in visual search , 1992, Nature.
[25] J. Wolfe,et al. The role of categorization in visual search for orientation. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[26] Eren Gunseli,et al. Is a search template an ordinary working memory? Comparing electrophysiological markers of working memory maintenance for visual search and recognition , 2014, Neuropsychologia.
[27] Yuhong Jiang,et al. Setting up the target template in visual search. , 2005, Journal of vision.
[28] A Treisman,et al. Feature analysis in early vision: evidence from search asymmetries. , 1988, Psychological review.
[29] Damien J. Mannion,et al. Orientation anisotropies in human visual cortex. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.
[30] G. Svaetichin,et al. Spectral response curves from single cones. , 1956, Acta physiologica Scandinavica. Supplementum.
[31] Colin W. G. Clifford,et al. Orientation anisotropies in human primary visual cortex depend on contrast , 2015, NeuroImage.
[32] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[33] W. Cowan,et al. Visual search for colour targets that are or are not linearly separable from distractors , 1996, Vision Research.
[34] D. Foster,et al. Asymmetries in oriented-line detection indicate two orthogonal filters in early vision , 1991, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[35] P Cavanagh,et al. Familiarity and pop-out in visual search , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.
[36] M. Eimer,et al. On the difference between working memory and attentional set , 2011, Neuropsychologia.
[37] J. Wolfe,et al. Which end is up? Two representations of orientation in visual search , 1999, Vision Research.
[38] P. Lennie,et al. Chromatic mechanisms in lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque. , 1984, The Journal of physiology.
[39] B. Julesz,et al. Human factors and behavioral science: Textons, the fundamental elements in preattentive vision and perception of textures , 1983, The Bell System Technical Journal.
[40] J. Theeuwes,et al. Feature-based memory-driven attentional capture: visual working memory content affects visual attention. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[41] J. Wolfe,et al. What attributes guide the deployment of visual attention and how do they do it? , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[42] Michael D'Zmura,et al. Color in visual search , 1991, Vision Research.
[43] J. Wolfe,et al. On the Role of Symmetry in Visual Search , 1992 .