Putting the horse before the cart: The use of temporal order in recall of events by one-year-old children.

Previous research has shown that 16- and 20-month-old children recall action sequences depicting familiar, script-based events, and also novel event sequences, in the correct temporal order. In 2 experiments, elicited imitation was used to assess 11.5- and 13.5-month-olds' immediate recall of familiar and novel event sequences. In Experiment 1,13.5-month-olds were tesfed on 2-act and 3-act sequences depicting both familiar and novel events. They reliably recalled the event sequences in the correct temporal order. In Experiment 2, the results were extended to 11.5-month-olds: They accurately recalled 2-act sequences depicting familiar and novel events. The results demonstrate that by late in the 1st year of life, children are able to accurately remember (a) specific sequences depicting familiar events and (b) novel event sequences. In contrast to traditional conceptualizations of preschoolers as unable to represent information in other than a piecemeal fashion (e.g., Fraisse, 1963; Piaget, 1926,1969), more recent research has shown that even young children recall both stories and personally experienced events in an organized manner similar to that of older children and adults (Mandler, 1978; Nelson, 1986). Like adults, in response to questions about what happens in the course of everyday events and routines, children as young as 3 recount events in their usual or canonical order (Nelson & Gruendel, 1981,1986). Although familiarity with events facilitates ordered recall (e.g, Fivush & Mandler, 1985), repeated experience is not necessary for recall to be temporally ordered. Three- to 7-year-olds provide well-ordered accounts of events after only a single experience of them (Hudson, 1986; B. S. Smith, Ratner, & Hobart, 1987). Furthermore, like older children and adults, preschoolers are sensitive to the structure of to-be-remembered material: They recall aspects of events with

[1]  J. Piaget The Language and Thought of the Child , 1927 .

[2]  P. Fraisse The psychology of time , 1963 .

[3]  Jean Piaget,et al.  The child's conception of time; , 1969 .

[4]  Mandler JeanM. A code in the node: The use of a story schema in retrieval , 1978 .

[5]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[6]  D. Ramsay,et al.  Decentration and Integration of the Child's Play in the Second Year. , 1980 .

[7]  R. Weisberg,et al.  The role of the object concept in visual tracking: child-like errors in adults. , 1981, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[8]  Ann L. Brown,et al.  Advances in developmental psychology , 1981 .

[9]  L. McCune-Nicolich Toward Symbolic Functioning: Structure of Early Pretend Games and Potential Parallels with Language. , 1981 .

[10]  D. Ramsay,et al.  Effects of modeling action sequences on the play of twelve-, fifteen, and nineteen-month-old children. , 1981, Child development.

[11]  J. Gruendel,et al.  Generalized event representations: basic building blocks of cognitive development , 1981 .

[12]  Katherine Nelson,et al.  Effects of script structure on children's story recall. , 1983 .

[13]  P. H. Smith Five-month-old infant recall and utilization of temporal organization , 1984 .

[14]  M. Moscovitch Infant memory : its relation to normal and pathological memory in humans and other animals , 1984 .

[15]  Jean M. Mandler,et al.  Representation and Recall in Infancy , 1984 .

[16]  Barbara O'connell,et al.  Scripts and Scraps: The Development of Sequential Understanding. , 1985 .

[17]  J M Mandler,et al.  Developmental changes in the understanding of temporal sequence. , 1985, Child development.

[18]  K. Nelson,et al.  Event knowledge : structure and function in development , 1986 .

[19]  R. Baillargeon Representing the existence and the location of hidden objects: Object permanence in 6- and 8-month-old infants , 1986, Cognition.

[20]  H. Ratner,et al.  The role of cuing and organization in children's memory for events , 1987 .

[21]  P. Bauer,et al.  Making a memorable event: Effects of familiarity and organization on young children's recall of action sequences , 1987 .

[22]  R. Baillargeon Young infants' reasoning about the physical and spatial properties of a hidden object , 1987 .

[23]  M. Haith,et al.  Expectation and Anticipation of Dynamic Visual Events by 3.5-Month-Old Babies. , 1988 .

[24]  A. Meltzoff Infant imitation and memory: nine-month-olds in immediate and deferred tests. , 1988, Child development.

[25]  A. Meltzoff Infant Imitation After a 1-Week Delay: Long-Term Memory for Novel Acts and Multiple Stimuli. , 1988, Developmental psychology.

[26]  J. Mandler,et al.  One thing follows another: Effects of temporal structure on 1- to 2-year-olds' recall of events. , 1989 .

[27]  R. Fivush,et al.  Time and again: effects of repetition and retention interval on 2 year olds' event recall. , 1989, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[28]  P. Bauer,et al.  Scripts or scraps: reconsidering the development of sequential understanding. , 1990, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[29]  A. Meltzoff Towards a Developmental Cognitive Science , 1990, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[30]  G. Goodman,et al.  Visiting the wizard: children's memory for a recurring event. , 1990, Child development.

[31]  Adèle,et al.  The Development and Neural Bases of Higher Cognitive Functions. A conference. May 20-24, 1989, Philadelphia, Pa. , 1990, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[32]  J M Mandler,et al.  Recall of Events by Preverbal Children a , 1990, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[33]  Thirteen- and Sixteen-Month-Olds' Long-Term Recall of Event Sequences. , 1991 .

[34]  Marshall M. Haith,et al.  Young Infants' Visual Expectations for Symmetric and Asymmetric Stimulus Sequences. , 1991 .

[35]  Patricia J. Bauer,et al.  Holding it all together: How enabling relations facilitate young children's event recall ☆ , 1992 .

[36]  R. Fivush,et al.  Knowing and remembering in young children , 1992 .