Template-to-distractor distinctiveness regulates visual search efficiency.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] M. Eimer. The neural basis of attentional control in visual search , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[2] J. Wolfe,et al. The role of categorization in visual search for orientation. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[3] C W ERIKSEN,et al. Location of objects in a visual display as a function of the number of dimensions on which the objects differ. , 1952, Journal of experimental psychology.
[4] Katharina N. Seidl-Rathkopf,et al. Automatic guidance of attention during real-world visual search , 2015, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[5] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. The prefrontal cortex and the executive control of attention , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.
[6] Alasdair D. F. Clarke,et al. Seeing Beyond Salience and Guidance: The Role of Bias and Decision in Visual Search , 2019, Vision.
[7] Eren Gunseli,et al. Local and interregional alpha EEG dynamics dissociate between memory for search and memory for recognition , 2017, NeuroImage.
[8] I. Biederman. Perceiving Real-World Scenes , 1972, Science.
[9] Taosheng Liu,et al. Neural Determinants of Task Performance during Feature-Based Attention in Human Cortex , 2018, eNeuro.
[10] H. Egeth,et al. Searching for conjunctively defined targets. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[11] L. Chelazzi,et al. Associative knowledge controls deployment of visual selective attention , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[12] A. Treisman,et al. A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.
[13] Antonio Torralba,et al. Contextual guidance of eye movements and attention in real-world scenes: the role of global features in object search. , 2006, Psychological review.
[14] C. Bundesen. A theory of visual attention. , 1990, Psychological review.
[15] Ken Nakayama,et al. Visual search for object categories is predicted by the representational architecture of high-level visual cortex. , 2017, Journal of neurophysiology.
[16] Nikolaus Kriegeskorte,et al. Unique semantic space in the brain of each beholder predicts perceived similarity , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[17] Joy J Geng,et al. Attentional capture by a perceptually salient non-target facilitates target processing through inhibition and rapid rejection. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[18] Susan L. Franzel,et al. Guided search: an alternative to the feature integration model for visual search. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[19] Clayton Hickey,et al. A temporal dependency account of attentional inhibition in oculomotor control , 2017, NeuroImage.
[20] Michael C Hout,et al. Target templates: the precision of mental representations affects attentional guidance and decision-making in visual search , 2015, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[21] Edward Awh,et al. Feature-Selective Attentional Modulations in Human Frontoparietal Cortex , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[22] G. Woodman,et al. Storage of features, conjunctions and objects in visual working memory. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[23] Hany Farid,et al. Observers change their target template based on expected context , 2016, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[24] J. Serences,et al. Optimal Deployment of Attentional Gain during Fine Discriminations , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[25] Gregory J Zelinsky,et al. Searching for Category-Consistent Features , 2016, Psychological science.
[26] Stefan Pollmann,et al. Neural correlates of visual dimension weighting , 2006 .
[27] Yaoda Xu,et al. Decoding the content of visual short-term memory under distraction in occipital and parietal areas , 2015, Nature Neuroscience.
[28] C. Bundesen,et al. A neural theory of visual attention: bridging cognition and neurophysiology. , 2005, Psychological review.
[29] Michael Lindenbaum,et al. Predicting visual search performance by quantifying stimuli similarities. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[30] R. Goebel,et al. Human Object-Similarity Judgments Reflect and Transcend the Primate-IT Object Representation , 2013, Front. Psychol..
[31] Stefan Pollmann,et al. Task relevance modulates the cortical representation of feature conjunctions in the target template , 2017, Scientific Reports.
[32] Stefanie I. Becker,et al. Visual search for color and shape: when is the gaze guided by feature relationships, when by feature values? , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[33] J. Serences,et al. Adaptive Allocation of Attentional Gain , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[34] Stefanie I. Becker,et al. The role of relational information in contingent capture. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[35] W. Cowan,et al. Visual search for colour targets that are or are not linearly separable from distractors , 1996, Vision Research.
[36] J. Ryan,et al. The intersection between the oculomotor and hippocampal memory systems: empirical developments and clinical implications , 2019, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[37] Robert G Alexander,et al. Visual similarity effects in categorical search. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[38] D. Heeger,et al. Categorical Clustering of the Neural Representation of Color , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[39] Lee M. Miller,et al. The Role of Alpha Activity in Spatial and Feature-Based Attention , 2016, eNeuro.
[40] B. Green,et al. Color coding in a visual search task. , 1956, Journal of experimental psychology.
[41] Martin Eimer,et al. Rapid guidance of visual search by object categories. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[42] J. Duncan,et al. Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.
[43] B. Anderson. On the precision of goal-directed attentional selection. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[44] J. Wolfe,et al. The role of memory for visual search in scenes , 2015, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[45] R. Nosofsky. Attention, similarity, and the identification-categorization relationship. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[46] Stefanie I. Becker. Determinants of Dwell Time in Visual Search: Similarity or Perceptual Difficulty? , 2011, PloS one.
[47] Monica S. Castelhano,et al. Peripheral guidance in scenes: The interaction of scene context and object content. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[48] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. Parallel Distractor Rejection as a Binding Mechanism in Search , 2012, Front. Psychology.
[49] Geoffrey F. Woodman,et al. Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[50] Daniel Baldauf,et al. Neural mechanisms of object-based attention , 2014 .
[51] J. Wolfe,et al. Five factors that guide attention in visual search , 2017, Nature Human Behaviour.
[52] George L. Malcolm,et al. The effects of target template specificity on visual search in real-world scenes: evidence from eye movements. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[53] Robert L. Goldstone,et al. Reuniting perception and conception , 1998, Cognition.
[54] A. Nagy,et al. Critical color differences determined with a visual search task. , 1990, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[55] Hany Farid,et al. Task Demands Determine the Specificity of the Search Template , 2022 .
[56] Eren Gunseli,et al. Effects of Search Difficulty on the Selection, Maintenance, and Learning of Attentional Templates , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[57] G W Humphreys,et al. Driving attention with the top down: The relative contribution of target templates to the linear separability effect in the size dimension , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[58] George L. Malcolm,et al. Combining top-down processes to guide eye movements during real-world scene search. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[59] Jonathan I. Flombaum,et al. Stimulus-specific variability in color working memory with delayed estimation. , 2014, Journal of vision.
[60] T. Moore,et al. The What and Where of Visual Attention , 2015, Neuron.
[61] Philip L. Smith,et al. An integrated theory of attention and decision making in visual signal detection. , 2009, Psychological review.
[62] Gregory J Zelinsky,et al. Adding Details to the Attentional Template Offsets Search Difficulty: Evidence From Contralateral Delay Activity , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[63] Jan Theeuwes,et al. Early and late modulation of saccade deviations by target distractor similarity. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.
[64] Richelle L. Witherspoon,et al. How You Use It Matters , 2016, Psychological science.
[65] Nikolaus Weiskopf,et al. Causal evidence for frontal involvement in memory target maintenance by posterior brain areas during distracter interference of visual working memory , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[66] Martin Eimer,et al. The Time Course of Target Template Activation Processes during Preparation for Visual Search , 2018, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[67] Joy J Geng,et al. Idiosyncratic Patterns of Representational Similarity in Prefrontal Cortex Predict Attentional Performance , 2017, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[68] P. Roelfsema,et al. Different States in Visual Working Memory: When It Guides Attention and When It Does Not , 2022 .
[69] L. Itti,et al. Search Goal Tunes Visual Features Optimally , 2007, Neuron.
[70] Edward Awh,et al. The contralateral delay activity as a neural measure of visual working memory , 2016, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[71] Martin Eimer,et al. Item and category-based attentional control during search for real-world objects: Can you find the pants among the pans? , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[72] Assaf Harel,et al. What is special about expertise? Visual expertise reveals the interactive nature of real-world object recognition , 2016, Neuropsychologia.
[73] H J Müller,et al. Visual search for singleton feature targets within and across feature dimensions , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.
[74] Andrew B. Leber,et al. Characterizing Individual Variation in the Strategic Use of Attentional Control , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[75] Anna C. Nobre,et al. Feature-based attentional weighting and spreading in visual working memory , 2017, Scientific Reports.
[76] Jessica L. Irons,et al. Choosing attentional control settings in a dynamically changing environment , 2016, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[77] H. Pashler,et al. Close binding of identity and location in visual feature perception. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[78] Nicole M. Long,et al. Bottom-Up and Top-Down Factors Differentially Influence Stimulus Representations Across Large-Scale Attentional Networks , 2018, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[79] Heleen A Slagter,et al. Inhibition in selective attention , 2020, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[80] Fred H. Hamker,et al. Open and closed loops: A computational approach to attention and consciousness , 2012, Advances in cognitive psychology.
[81] Marius V Peelen,et al. The contents of the search template for category-level search in natural scenes. , 2013, Journal of vision.
[82] Joy J Geng,et al. Distractor Probability Changes the Shape of the Attentional Template , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[83] N. Kriegeskorte,et al. Inverse MDS: Inferring Dissimilarity Structure from Multiple Item Arrangements , 2012, Front. Psychology.
[84] Stefanie I. Becker,et al. Oculomotor capture by colour singletons depends on intertrial priming , 2010, Vision Research.
[85] S P Arun,et al. Does linear separability really matter? Complex visual search is explained by simple search. , 2013, Journal of vision.
[86] J. Geng,et al. The Attentional Template Is Shifted and Asymmetrically Sharpened by Distractor Context , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[87] Tamaryn Menneer,et al. Using multidimensional scaling to quantify similarity in visual search and beyond , 2015, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[88] R. Desimone,et al. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.
[89] Jeremy M. Wolfe,et al. Guided Search 4.0: Current Progress With a Model of Visual Search , 2007, Integrated Models of Cognitive Systems.