Comparable search efficiency for human and animal targets in the context of natural scenes
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Raja Parasuraman,et al. Attention, biological motion, and action recognition , 2012, NeuroImage.
[2] M. Shiffrar,et al. The visual perception of human locomotion. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[3] Quoc C. Vuong,et al. Humans are Detected More Efficiently than Machines in the Context of Natural Scenes , 2017 .
[4] Eric Hiris,et al. Detection of biological and nonbiological motion. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[5] G. Mather,et al. Recognition of Animal Locomotion from Dynamic Point-Light Displays , 1993, Perception.
[6] T. Poggio,et al. Cognitive neuroscience: Neural mechanisms for the recognition of biological movements , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[7] J. Cutting,et al. Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display , 1977 .
[8] J. Wolfe,et al. Five factors that guide attention in visual search , 2017, Nature Human Behaviour.
[9] Muriel Boucart,et al. Finding faces, animals, and vehicles in far peripheral vision. , 2016, Journal of vision.
[10] Junpeng Lao,et al. Residual perception of biological motion in cortical blindness , 2016, Neuropsychologia.
[11] J ValdésJulio,et al. 2006 Special issue , 2006 .
[12] L. Cosmides,et al. Category-specific attention for animals reflects ancestral priorities, not expertise , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[13] R. Gregory,et al. HUMAN PERCEPTION. , 1964, British medical bulletin.
[14] L. Gogate,et al. Attention and memory for faces and actions in infancy: the salience of actions over faces in dynamic events. , 2002, Child development.
[15] A. Treisman,et al. Search asymmetry: a diagnostic for preattentive processing of separable features. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[16] J. Duncan,et al. Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.
[17] A. Treisman,et al. A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.
[18] E. Erdfelder,et al. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses , 2009, Behavior research methods.
[19] Jim W Kay,et al. Gender recognition from point-light walkers. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[20] J A Beintema,et al. Perception of biological motion without local image motion , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[21] J. Wolfe,et al. What Can 1 Million Trials Tell Us About Visual Search? , 1998 .
[22] Ronald A. Rensink,et al. Active versus passive processing of biological motion , 2002, Perception.
[23] R. Blake,et al. Brain Areas Involved in Perception of Biological Motion , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[24] N. Bolger,et al. Human Perception of Fear in Dogs Varies According to Experience with Dogs , 2012, PloS one.
[25] R. Blake,et al. Perception of human motion. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.
[26] Christoph Scheepers,et al. Face, body, and center of gravity mediate person detection in natural scenes. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[27] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. Distinct Regions of Right Temporal Cortex Are Associated with Biological and Human–Agent Motion: Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Neuropsychological Evidence , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[28] J. Wolfe,et al. What attributes guide the deployment of visual attention and how do they do it? , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[29] Jeroen J. A. van Boxtel,et al. Signature Movements Lead to Efficient Search for Threatening Actions , 2012, PloS one.
[30] Pawan Sinha,et al. Top-down influences on stereoscopic depth-perception , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.
[31] R. Blake,et al. Perception of Biological Motion , 1997, Perception.
[32] J Faubert,et al. Independence of Contour and Biological-Motion Cues for Motion-Defined Animal Shapes , 1998, Perception.
[33] Lara Bardi,et al. Biological motion preference in humans at birth: role of dynamic and configural properties. , 2011, Developmental science.
[34] Miguel P Eckstein,et al. Visual search: a retrospective. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[35] Christof Koch,et al. Modeling attention to salient proto-objects , 2006, Neural Networks.
[36] J. Lange,et al. A Model of Biological Motion Perception from Configural Form Cues , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[37] B. Bertenthal,et al. Infant sensitivity to figural coherence in biomechanical motions. , 1984, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[38] A. Saygin. Superior temporal and premotor brain areas necessary for biological motion perception. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[39] G. Johansson. Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .
[40] A. Clough,et al. Integrated computational model of the bioenergetics of isolated lung mitochondria , 2018, PloS one.
[41] Q. Vuong,et al. Incidental Processing of Biological Motion , 2004, Current Biology.
[42] Maggie Shiffrar,et al. Socially tuned: Brain responses differentiating human and animal motion , 2012, Social neuroscience.
[43] P. Downing,et al. Bodies capture attention when nothing is expected , 2004, Cognition.
[44] J. Deloache,et al. Detecting the Snake in the Grass , 2008, Psychological science.
[45] F. Simion,et al. A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[46] P. Cavanagh,et al. Attention-based visual routines: sprites , 2001, Cognition.
[47] A. Ohman,et al. Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[48] Sheng He,et al. Biological motion cues trigger reflexive attentional orienting , 2010, Cognition.
[49] N. Troje,et al. The Inversion Effect in Biological Motion Perception: Evidence for a “Life Detector”? , 2006, Current Biology.
[50] Katja M. Mayer,et al. Do People “Pop Out”? , 2015, PloS one.
[51] R. Fox,et al. The perception of biological motion by human infants. , 1982, Science.
[52] L. M. M.-T.. Theory of Probability , 1929, Nature.
[53] E. Wagenmakers,et al. Why psychologists must change the way they analyze their data: the case of psi: comment on Bem (2011). , 2011, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[54] M. Shiffrar,et al. The visual perception of human and animal motion in point-light displays , 2009, Social neuroscience.
[55] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[56] Denis G. Pelli,et al. ECVP '07 Abstracts , 2007, Perception.
[57] N. Birbaumer,et al. Recognition of Point-Light Biological Motion Displays by Young Children , 2001, Perception.
[58] Miiamaaria V. Kujala,et al. Dog Experts' Brains Distinguish Socially Relevant Body Postures Similarly in Dogs and Humans , 2012, PloS one.
[59] M. Giese,et al. Learning to discriminate complex movements: biological versus artificial trajectories. , 2006, Journal of vision.
[60] Derrick J. Parkhurst,et al. Modeling the role of salience in the allocation of overt visual attention , 2002, Vision Research.
[61] Moritz F. Wurm,et al. The neural representation of human versus nonhuman bipeds and quadrupeds , 2017, Scientific Reports.
[62] S Ullman,et al. Shifts in selective visual attention: towards the underlying neural circuitry. , 1985, Human neurobiology.
[63] John A. Pyles,et al. Visual perception and neural correlates of novel ‘biological motion’ , 2007, Vision Research.
[64] D G Pelli,et al. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.