Naturalistic Stimulus Structure Determines the Integration of Audiovisual Looming Signals in Binocular Rivalry

Rapid integration of biologically relevant information is crucial for the survival of an organism. Most prominently, humans should be biased to attend and respond to looming stimuli that signal approaching danger (e.g. predator) and hence require rapid action. This psychophysics study used binocular rivalry to investigate the perceptual advantage of looming (relative to receding) visual signals (i.e. looming bias) and how this bias can be influenced by concurrent auditory looming/receding stimuli and the statistical structure of the auditory and visual signals. Subjects were dichoptically presented with looming/receding visual stimuli that were paired with looming or receding sounds. The visual signals conformed to two different statistical structures: (1) a ‘simple’ random-dot kinematogram showing a starfield and (2) a “naturalistic” visual Shepard stimulus. Likewise, the looming/receding sound was (1) a simple amplitude- and frequency-modulated (AM-FM) tone or (2) a complex Shepard tone. Our results show that the perceptual looming bias (i.e. the increase in dominance times for looming versus receding percepts) is amplified by looming sounds, yet reduced and even converted into a receding bias by receding sounds. Moreover, the influence of looming/receding sounds on the visual looming bias depends on the statistical structure of both the visual and auditory signals. It is enhanced when audiovisual signals are Shepard stimuli. In conclusion, visual perception prioritizes processing of biologically significant looming stimuli especially when paired with looming auditory signals. Critically, these audiovisual interactions are amplified for statistically complex signals that are more naturalistic and known to engage neural processing at multiple levels of the cortical hierarchy.

[1]  John G. Neuhoff,et al.  An Adaptive Bias in the Perception of Looming Auditory Motion , 2001 .

[2]  U. Noppeney,et al.  Perceptual Decisions Formed by Accumulation of Audiovisual Evidence in Prefrontal Cortex , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  Michael S. Lewicki,et al.  Efficient coding of natural sounds , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[4]  Gregor Thut,et al.  Selective integration of auditory-visual looming cues by humans , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  Joost X. Maier,et al.  Natural, Metaphoric, and Linguistic Auditory Direction Signals Have Distinct Influences on Visual Motion Processing , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  Antonio Torralba,et al.  Statistics of natural image categories , 2003, Network.

[7]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Natural image statistics and neural representation. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[8]  J. Petersen,et al.  Conjugation in S. pombe: identification of a microtubule-organising centre, a requirement for microtubules and a role for Mad2 , 1998, Current Biology.

[9]  W Schiff,et al.  Accuracy of judging time to arrival: effects of modality, trajectory, and gender. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  Christoph Kayser,et al.  Spatial Organization of Multisensory Responses in Temporal Association Cortex , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  A. Ghazanfar,et al.  Is neocortex essentially multisensory? , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  Daniel R. Berger,et al.  Sensor fusion in the perception of self motion , 2007 .

[13]  Daniel R. Berger Spectral texturing for real-time applications , 2003, SIGGRAPH '03.

[14]  Erich W Graf,et al.  Natural images dominate in binocular rivalry , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[15]  Asif A Ghazanfar,et al.  Multisensory Integration of Looming Signals by Rhesus Monkeys , 2004, Neuron.

[16]  Fabrizio Gabbiani,et al.  Collision detection as a model for sensory-motor integration. , 2011, Annual review of neuroscience.

[17]  Natalie M. Myres,et al.  Distinctive Paleo-Indian Migration Routes from Beringia Marked by Two Rare mtDNA Haplogroups , 2009, Current Biology.

[18]  David Alais,et al.  Strength and coherence of binocular rivalry depends on shared stimulus complexity , 2007, Vision Research.

[19]  K. G. Munhall,et al.  Audiovisual Integration of Speech in a Bistable Illusion , 2009, Current Biology.

[20]  C. Clifford,et al.  Perceptual Grouping of Biological Motion Promotes Binocular Rivalry , 2004, Current Biology.

[21]  Katie L. H. Gray,et al.  High-Level Face Adaptation Without Awareness , 2010, Psychological science.

[22]  G. Rees,et al.  The Neural Bases of Multistable Perception , 2022 .

[23]  David Alais,et al.  Multisensory Congruency as a Mechanism for Attentional Control over Perceptual Selection , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  R. Blake,et al.  Endogenous attention prolongs dominance durations in binocular rivalry. , 2005, Journal of vision.

[25]  T. Stanford,et al.  Multisensory integration: current issues from the perspective of the single neuron , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[26]  Israel Nelken,et al.  Responses of auditory-cortex neurons to structural features of natural sounds , 1999, Nature.

[27]  David Alais,et al.  A bias for looming stimuli to predominate in binocular rivalry , 2007, Vision Research.

[28]  A. King,et al.  Unraveling the principles of auditory cortical processing: can we learn from the visual system? , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[29]  D. Lewkowicz Perception of dynamic and static audiovisual sequences in 3- and 4-month-old infants. , 2008, Child development.

[30]  G. J. Burton,et al.  Color and spatial structure in natural scenes. , 1987, Applied optics.

[31]  Asif A. Ghazanfar,et al.  Integration of Bimodal Looming Signals through Neuronal Coherence in the Temporal Lobe , 2008, Current Biology.

[32]  Dylan F. Cooke,et al.  Parieto-frontal interactions, personal space, and defensive behavior , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  D J Field,et al.  Relations between the statistics of natural images and the response properties of cortical cells. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[34]  Erich Seifritz,et al.  Looming sounds as warning signals: the function of motion cues. , 2009, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[35]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[36]  D G Pelli,et al.  Pixel independence: measuring spatial interactions on a CRT display. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[37]  Mikko Sams,et al.  Audio–visual speech perception is special , 2005, Cognition.

[38]  L. Rosenblum,et al.  Relative Effectiveness of Three Stimulus Variables for Locating a Moving Sound Source , 1987, Perception.

[39]  U. Noppeney,et al.  Audiovisual Synchrony Improves Motion Discrimination via Enhanced Connectivity between Early Visual and Auditory Areas , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  Dylan F. Cooke,et al.  Parieto-frontal interactions, personal space, and defensive behavior , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  Rainer Guski,et al.  Acoustic Tau: An Easy Analogue to Visual Tau? , 1992 .

[42]  U. Noppeney,et al.  Distinct Functional Contributions of Primary Sensory and Association Areas to Audiovisual Integration in Object Categorization , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  Charles Spence,et al.  ‘When Birds of a Feather Flock Together’: Synesthetic Correspondences Modulate Audiovisual Integration in Non-Synesthetes , 2009, PloS one.

[44]  Uta Noppeney,et al.  Physical and Perceptual Factors Shape the Neural Mechanisms That Integrate Audiovisual Signals in Speech Comprehension , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  R. Shepard Circularity in Judgments of Relative Pitch , 1964 .

[46]  Dylan F. Cooke,et al.  Defensive movements evoked by air puff in monkeys. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  Asif A Ghazanfar,et al.  Looming Biases in Monkey Auditory Cortex , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  M. Murray,et al.  Looming Signals Reveal Synergistic Principles of Multisensory Integration , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  J. Rauschecker,et al.  Cortical Representation of Natural Complex Sounds: Effects of Acoustic Features and Auditory Object Category , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[50]  Denis G. Pelli,et al.  ECVP '07 Abstracts , 2007, Perception.

[51]  John G. Neuhoff,et al.  Perceptual bias for rising tones , 1998, Nature.

[52]  Christoph Kayser,et al.  Suppressive Competition: How Sounds May Cheat Sight , 2012, Neuron.

[53]  D. Tolhurst,et al.  Amplitude spectra of natural images. , 1992, Ophthalmic & physiological optics : the journal of the British College of Ophthalmic Opticians.

[54]  David J. Field,et al.  What Is the Goal of Sensory Coding? , 1994, Neural Computation.

[55]  I. Kovács,et al.  When the brain changes its mind: interocular grouping during binocular rivalry. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.