Independent Processing of Form, Colour, and Texture in Object Perception

Most investigations of object recognition have focused on the form rather than the material properties of objects. Nevertheless, knowledge of the material properties of an object (via its surface cues) can provide important information about that object's identity. In this study, we used Garner's speeded-classification task to explore whether or not the processing of form and the processing of surface properties are independent. In experiment 1, participants made length and width classifications in an initial form task. Participants were unable to ignore length while making width classifications, and were unable to ignore width while making length classifications. This suggests that the perception of length and the perception of width share common processing resources. In a subsequent task, we examined possible interactions between the processing of form and the processing of surface properties. In contrast to the findings with the form task, participants were able to ignore form while making surface-property classifications, and to ignore surface properties while making form classifications. This suggests that the form of objects and their surface properties are processed independently. In experiment 2, we went on to show that the two prominent surface-property dimensions of colour and texture can also be processed independently. In other words, participants were able to ignore colour while making texture classifications, and vice versa. Finally, in experiment 3, we examined the possibility that the stimuli and required responses that we used in experiment 2 were too categorical and thus not optimal for assessing whether or not colour and texture share common processing resources. Using a different stimulus set, participants were again able to ignore colour while making texture classifications, and vice versa. Taken together, these results provided convincing evidence that the separate ventral-stream brain regions identified for form, texture, and colour in a recent neuroimaging study (Cant and Goodale, 2007 Cerebral Cortex 17 713–731) can indeed function independently.

[1]  M. Goodale,et al.  Visual control of action but not perception requires analytical processing of object shape , 2003, Nature.

[2]  A. Chaudhuri,et al.  Recognizing faces defined by texture gradients , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[3]  W. R. Garner,et al.  Effects of focusing strategy on speeded classification with grouping, filtering, and condensation tasks , 1972 .

[4]  N. Macmillan,et al.  The mean-integral representation of rectangles , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.

[5]  Edward H. Adelson,et al.  On seeing stuff: the perception of materials by humans and machines , 2001, IS&T/SPIE Electronic Imaging.

[6]  M. Vorobyev,et al.  Colour vision as an adaptation to frugivory in primates , 1996, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[7]  M. Goodale,et al.  Sight Unseen: An Exploration of Conscious and Unconscious Vision , 2004 .

[8]  M. Goodale,et al.  Ventral occipital lesions impair object recognition but not object-directed grasping: an fMRI study. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  R. Malach,et al.  Object-related activity revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital cortex. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[10]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Behavioral and Neuroimaging Evidence for a Contribution of Color and Texture Information to Scene Classification in a Patient with Visual Form Agnosia , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  The human visual cortex. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[12]  DH Hubel,et al.  Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[13]  Karl Magnus Petersson,et al.  Color makes a difference: Two-dimensional object naming in literate and illiterate subjects , 2006, Brain and Cognition.

[14]  J R Dykes,et al.  An investigation of the perceptual basis of redundancy gain and orthogonal interference for integral dimensions , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[15]  P Møller,et al.  Psychophysical evidence for fast region-based segmentation processes in motion and color. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  Shaul Hochstein,et al.  Interactions in the Discrimination and Absolute Judgement of Orientation and Length , 1988, Perception.

[17]  Stephen P. Harshfield,et al.  Weight judgment as a function of apparent density of objects , 1970 .

[18]  P Corcia,et al.  [Cerebral achromatopsia without prosopagnosia, alexia, object agnosia]. , 1997, Revue neurologique.

[19]  A. Oliva,et al.  Coarse Blobs or Fine Edges? Evidence That Information Diagnosticity Changes the Perception of Complex Visual Stimuli , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[20]  B. Rossion,et al.  Revisiting Snodgrass and Vanderwart's Object Pictorial Set: The Role of Surface Detail in Basic-Level Object Recognition , 2004, Perception.

[21]  Ioannis Patras,et al.  Combining color and shape information for illumination-viewpoint invariant object recognition , 2006, IEEE Transactions on Image Processing.

[22]  S. Zeki,et al.  The position and topography of the human colour centre as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[23]  Robert W. Kentridge,et al.  Achromatopsia, color vision, and cortex. , 2003, Neurologic clinics.

[24]  G. Orban,et al.  Attention to 3-D Shape, 3-D Motion, and Texture in 3-D Structure from Motion Displays , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  Daniel Kersten,et al.  Is Color an Intrinsic Property of Object Representation? , 2003, Perception.

[26]  S. Edelman,et al.  Cue-Invariant Activation in Object-Related Areas of the Human Occipital Lobe , 1998, Neuron.

[27]  Jonathan S. Cant,et al.  Cerebral Cortex Advance Access published April 28, 2006 Attention to Form or Surface Properties Modulates Different Regions of Human , 2022 .

[28]  A. Oliva,et al.  Flexible, Diagnosticity-Driven, Rather Than Fixed, Perceptually Determined Scale Selection in Scene and Face Recognition , 1997, Perception.

[29]  T. R. Jordan,et al.  Perception and action in 'visual form agnosia'. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[30]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Integration of Local Features into Global Shapes Monkey and Human fMRI Studies , 2003, Neuron.

[31]  A. Oliva,et al.  Diagnostic Colors Mediate Scene Recognition , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[32]  Anil K. Jain,et al.  On image classification: city images vs. landscapes , 1998, Pattern Recognit..

[33]  Tutis Vilis,et al.  Perceptual continuity and the emergence of perceptual persistence in the ventral visual pathway. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  W. R. Garner The Processing of Information and Structure , 1974 .

[35]  G W Humphreys,et al.  Integration of Physical and Semantic Information in Object Processing , 1997, Perception.

[36]  Shaul Hochstein,et al.  Asymmetric Interactions in the Processing of the Visual Dimensions of Position, Width, and Contrast of Bar Stimuli , 1992, Perception.

[37]  A. Cowey,et al.  Cerebral Achromatopsia in Monkeys , 1995, The European journal of neuroscience.

[38]  Rebecca F. Schwarzlose,et al.  Separate face and body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus. , 2010, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[39]  D. Weiskopf,et al.  The role of color in high-level vision , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[40]  M. Wong-Riley,et al.  Primate Visual Cortex , 1994 .

[41]  I. Biederman,et al.  Scene perception: Detecting and judging objects undergoing relational violations , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[42]  Antonio Torralba,et al.  Modeling the Shape of the Scene: A Holistic Representation of the Spatial Envelope , 2001, International Journal of Computer Vision.

[43]  I. Biederman,et al.  Is Pigmentation Important for Face Recognition? Evidence from Contrast Negation , 2006, Perception.

[44]  Doris Y. Tsao,et al.  Neuroimaging Weighs In: Humans Meet Macaques in “Primate” Visual Cortex , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  Richard C. Clarke,et al.  Seeing in colour , 2011, Nature.

[46]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  High-resolution imaging reveals highly selective nonface clusters in the fusiform face area , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[47]  G K Humphrey,et al.  The Role of Surface Information in Object Recognition: Studies of a Visual Form Agnosic and Normal Subjects , 1994, Perception.

[48]  G W Humphreys,et al.  Attention to orientation, size, luminance, and color: attentional failure within the form domain. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[49]  J. Tanaka,et al.  Color diagnosticity in object recognition , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[50]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Retinotopy and color sensitivity in human visual cortical area V8 , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[51]  Bruno Rossion,et al.  Men are from Mars, women are from Venus: Behavioral and neural correlates of face sexing using color , 2010 .

[52]  H. Wilson,et al.  Detection of global structure in Glass patterns: implications for form vision , 1998, Vision Research.

[53]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Global shape cannot be attended without object identification. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[54]  Bruno Rossion,et al.  It's Pat! Sexing faces using only red and green , 2010 .

[55]  J. Rieger,et al.  Sensory and cognitive contributions of color to the recognition of natural scenes , 2000, Current Biology.

[56]  A. Oliva,et al.  From Blobs to Boundary Edges: Evidence for Time- and Spatial-Scale-Dependent Scene Recognition , 1994 .

[57]  Gary L. Felfoldy Repetition effects in choice reaction time to multidimensional stimuli , 1974 .

[58]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Texture segregation in the human visual cortex: A functional MRI study. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.