Video game designs by girls and boys: variability and consistency of gender differences

Over the past ten years interactive technologies have become a significant part of children’s culture. Video games such as Super Mario BrothersTM or SonicTM have found a stable place in children’s playrooms, in particular games catering to boy’s interests in sports, adventure and combat (Provenzo, 1991). Only recently has so called “pink software” established a playground for girls promoting games and software such as Barbie’s Fashion DesignerTM or the Babysitter ClubTM that draw on characters and activities popular among girls. In many ways, the production of interactive toys and games seems to replicate gender differences found in traditional toys and games and the interests these generate in children (Garvey, 1990; Kinder, 1991; Singer & Singer, 1991; Sutton-Smith, 1986). There is ample evidence in the research literature for the existence of gender differences in children’s video game interest, use and performance (Goldstein, 1994; Provenzo, 1991). These gender differences also appear when children are asked to make their own video games (Kafai, 1996). But there are some indicators that these differences are not as universal as they may appear at first: some software such as Where in the World is Carmen Sandiego?TM seem to have equal appeal for boys and girls and some girls like to play video games albeit with different interpretations (Gailey, 1993). Furthermore, gender differences in play performance disappear after extended exposure (Greenfield & Cocking, 1994), a claim which is also supported by research on girls’ general use of and interest in technology (Linn, 1985). While these are isolated indicators, they point out that gender differences are not as consistent as one might believe. It is possible that children display more versatility and range in their play interests and

[1]  J. Piaget Play, dreams and imitation in childhood , 1951 .

[2]  J. Bruner,et al.  Play: Its Role in Development and Evolution , 1976 .

[3]  W. Montague,et al.  Aptitude, Learning, and Instruction , 1980 .

[4]  R. Olney,et al.  The effect of boys' or girls' toys on sex-typed play in preadolescents , 1983 .

[5]  Patricia M. Greenfield,et al.  Mind and Media: The Effects of Television, Video Games, and Computers , 1984 .

[6]  Sherry Turkle,et al.  The second self: computers and the human spirit , 1984 .

[7]  Marcia C. Linn,et al.  Fostering equitable consequences from computer learning environments , 1985 .

[8]  B. Sutton-Smith,et al.  Toys As Culture , 1986 .

[9]  S. Silvern,et al.  The effects of video game play on young children's aggression, fantasy, and prosocial behavior , 1987 .

[10]  J. Cooper,et al.  Sex Bias in Educational Software: The Effect of Designers' Stereotypes on the Software They Design1 , 1987 .

[11]  H. Ross,et al.  Do boys prefer daddy or his physical style of play? , 1989 .

[12]  R. Larson,et al.  The Use and Experience of the New Video Media Among Children and Young Adolescents , 1990 .

[13]  Eugene F. Provenzo,et al.  Video Kids: Making Sense of Nintendo , 1991 .

[14]  Book reviewThe house of make-believe: Play and the developing imagination: Singer, D.G., & Singer, J.L. Cambridge, Ma: Harvard University Press, 1990 , 1992 .

[15]  Kenneth H. Rubin,et al.  Sex typing in play and popularity in middle childhood , 1992 .

[16]  Yasmin B. Kafai,et al.  Minds In Play: Computer Game Design as a Context for Children''s , 1994 .

[17]  C. Gailey Mediated Messages: Gender, Class, and Cosmos in Home Video Games , 1993 .

[18]  Kori M. Inkpen,et al.  We Have Never-Forgetful Flowers In Our Garden: Girls'' Responses to Electronic Games , 1993 .

[19]  J. Goldstein,et al.  Toys, Play, and Child Development , 1994 .

[20]  Patricia M. Greenfield,et al.  Video games as cultural artifacts. , 1994 .

[21]  P. Greenfield,et al.  Action video games and informal education: Effects on strategies for dividing visual attention , 1994 .

[22]  P. Frensch,et al.  Effects of video game playing on measures of spatial performance: Gender effects in late adolescence. , 1994 .

[23]  Patricia M. Greenfield,et al.  Two-dimensional representation of movement through three- dimensional space: The role of video game expertise☆ , 1994 .

[24]  P. Greenfield,et al.  Cognitive socialization by computer games in two cultures: inductive discovery or mastery of an iconic code , 1994 .

[25]  Patricia M. Greenfield,et al.  Effect of video game practice on spatial skills in girls and boys , 1994 .

[26]  Patricia M. Greenfield,et al.  Effects of interactive entertainment technologies on development , 1994 .

[27]  S. Turkle Life on the Screen , 1995 .

[28]  Y. Kafai Gender Differences in Children's Constructions of Video Games , 1996 .

[29]  Yasmin B. Kafai,et al.  Interacting with Video , 1996 .