Otolithic information is required for homing in the mouse

Navigation and the underlying brain signals are influenced by various allothetic and idiothetic cues, depending on environmental conditions and task demands. Visual landmarks typically control navigation in familiar environments but, in the absence of landmarks, self‐movement cues are able to guide navigation relatively accurately. These self‐movement cues include signals from the vestibular system, and may originate in the semicircular canals or otolith organs. Here, we tested the otolithic contribution to navigation on a food‐hoarding task in darkness and in light. The dark test prevented the use of visual cues and thus favored the use of self‐movement information, whereas the light test allowed the use of both visual and non‐visual cues. In darkness, tilted mice made shorter‐duration stops during the outward journey, and made more circuitous homeward journeys than control mice; heading error, trip duration, and peak error were greater for tilted mice than for controls. In light, tilted mice also showed more circuitous homeward trips, but appeared to correct for errors during the journey; heading error, trip duration, and peak error were similar between groups. These results suggest that signals from the otolith organs are necessary for accurate homing performance in mice, with the greatest contribution in non‐visual environments. © 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

[1]  J. Bassett,et al.  Passive transport disrupts directional path integration by rat head direction cells. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conjunctive Representation of Position, Direction, and Velocity in Entorhinal Cortex , 2006 .

[3]  Sidney B. Williams,et al.  Directional Responding of C57BL/6J Mice in the Morris Water Maze Is Influenced by Visual and Vestibular Cues and Is Dependent on the Anterior Thalamic Nuclei , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  Alain Berthoz,et al.  Enhanced hippocampal theta EEG during whole body rotations in awake restrained rats , 1995, Neuroscience Letters.

[5]  Torkel Hafting,et al.  Conjunctive Representation of Position, Direction, and Velocity in Entorhinal Cortex , 2006, Science.

[6]  R. Muller,et al.  The effects of changes in the environment on the spatial firing of hippocampal complex-spike cells , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[7]  Ian Q. Whishaw,et al.  Path Integration Absent in Scent-Tracking Fimbria–Fornix Rats: Evidence for Hippocampal Involvement in “Sense of Direction” and “Sense of Distance” Using Self-Movement Cues , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  I. Whishaw,et al.  NMDA lesions of Ammon's horn and the dentate gyrus disrupt the direct and temporally paced homing displayed by rats exploring a novel environment: evidence for a role of the hippocampus in dead reckoning , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[9]  Mice use start point orientation to solve spatial problems in a water T-maze , 2014, Animal Cognition.

[10]  Douglas G. Wallace,et al.  Medial septum lesions disrupt exploratory trip organization: Evidence for septohippocampal involvement in dead reckoning , 2007, Physiology & Behavior.

[11]  Douglas G Wallace,et al.  Vestibular Information Is Required for Dead Reckoning in the Rat , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  永福 智志 The Organization of Learning , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  J. Taube,et al.  Head direction cell activity monitored in a novel environment and during a cue conflict situation. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  Ian Q. Whishaw,et al.  Food carrying: a new method for naturalistic studies of spontaneous and forced alternation , 1995, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[15]  J. O’Keefe,et al.  Single unit activity in the rat hippocampus during a spatial memory task , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  D. Wallace,et al.  Selective hippocampal cholinergic deafferentation impairs self-movement cue use during a food hoarding task , 2007, Behavioural Brain Research.

[17]  Seth L. Kirby,et al.  Otoconia‐deficient mice show selective spatial deficits , 2014, Hippocampus.

[18]  Passive rotation‐induced theta rhythm and orientation homeostasis response , 2010, Synapse.

[19]  C. Gallistel The organization of learning , 1990 .

[20]  R. Stackman,et al.  Rats with lesions of the vestibular system require a visual landmark for spatial navigation , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.

[21]  I. Whishaw,et al.  “Short‐stops” in rats with fimbria‐fornix lesions: Evidence for change in the mobility gradient , 1994, Hippocampus.

[22]  J. Taube,et al.  Head Direction Cell Activity in Mice: Robust Directional Signal Depends on Intact Otolith Organs , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  B. McNaughton,et al.  Spatial information content and reliability of hippocampal CA1 neurons: Effects of visual input , 1994, Hippocampus.

[24]  David K Bilkey,et al.  Lesions of the vestibular system disrupt hippocampal theta rhythm in the rat. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  R. Muller,et al.  Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. II. Effects of environmental manipulations , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[26]  J. Taube,et al.  Hippocampal spatial representations require vestibular input , 2002, Hippocampus.

[27]  D. Olton,et al.  Spatial memory and hippocampal function , 1979, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  Yiwen Zheng,et al.  Long‐term deficits on a foraging task after bilateral vestibular deafferentation in rats , 2009, Hippocampus.

[29]  Jenny R. Köppen,et al.  Infusion of GAT1-saporin into the medial septum/vertical limb of the diagonal band disrupts self-movement cue processing and spares mnemonic function , 2013, Brain Structure and Function.

[30]  Jeffrey S Taube,et al.  Path integration and lesions within the head direction cell circuit: comparison between the roles of the anterodorsal thalamus and dorsal tegmental nucleus. , 2006, Behavioral neuroscience.

[31]  Edvard I Moser,et al.  A metric for space , 2008, Hippocampus.

[32]  R U Muller,et al.  Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. I. Description and quantitative analysis , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[33]  Jenny R. Köppen,et al.  Navigating with fingers and feet: analysis of human (Homo sapiens) and rat (Rattus norvegicus) movement organization during nonvisual spatial tasks. , 2010, Journal of comparative psychology.

[34]  H. Mittelstaedt,et al.  Homing by path integration in a mammal , 1980, Naturwissenschaften.

[35]  I. Whishaw,et al.  Piloting and dead reckoning dissociated by fimbria-fornix lesions in a rat food carrying task , 1997, Behavioural Brain Research.

[36]  K. Pang,et al.  Involvement of GABAergic and cholinergic medial septal neurons in hippocampal theta rhythm , 2005, Hippocampus.

[37]  Carol A. Barnes,et al.  Head-direction cells in the rat posterior cortex , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  G. W. Harding,et al.  Otoconial agenesis in tilted mutant mice , 1998, Hearing Research.

[39]  K P Ossenkopp,et al.  Spatial learning in an enclosed eight-arm radial maze in rats with sodium arsanilate-induced labyrinthectomies. , 1993, Behavioral and neural biology.

[40]  P. Denise,et al.  Influence of vestibular input on spatial and nonspatial memory and on hippocampal NMDA receptors , 2012, Hippocampus.

[41]  Stefan Glasauer,et al.  Idiothetic navigation in Gerbils and Humans , 1991 .

[42]  B. J. Clark,et al.  Both visual and idiothetic cues contribute to head direction cell stability during navigation along complex routes. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  Katherine G Akers,et al.  How do room and apparatus cues control navigation in the Morris water task? Evidence for distinct contributions to a movement vector. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[44]  Douglas G Wallace,et al.  Comparative analysis of movement characteristics during dead-reckoning-based navigation in humans and rats. , 2006, Journal of comparative psychology.

[45]  J. Taube The head direction signal: origins and sensory-motor integration. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[46]  M. Mintz,et al.  Mice with vestibular deficiency display hyperactivity, disorientation, and signs of anxiety , 2009, Behavioural Brain Research.

[47]  Noah A. Russell,et al.  Long-Term Effects of Permanent Vestibular Lesions on Hippocampal Spatial Firing , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  C. H. Vanderwolf,et al.  Hippocampal electrical activity and voluntary movement in the rat. , 1969, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[49]  J. Baker,et al.  The vestibulo ocular reflex (VOR) in otoconia deficient head tilt (het) mutant mice versus wild type C57BL/6 mice , 2003, Brain Research.

[50]  E. J. Green,et al.  Head-direction cells in the rat posterior cortex , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[51]  Travis E. Johnson,et al.  The relative influence of place and direction in the Morris water task. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[52]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Spatial Representation in the Entorhinal Cortex , 2004 .

[53]  D. M. Skinner,et al.  Rats' orientation is more important than start point location for successful place learning. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[54]  J. O’Keefe,et al.  Hippocampal place units in the freely moving rat: Why they fire where they fire , 1978, Experimental Brain Research.

[55]  S. H Alyan,et al.  Interplay of directional navigation mechanisms as a function of near-goal distance: experiments with the house mouse , 1997, Behavioural Processes.

[56]  Shawn S. Winter,et al.  Mammillothalamic tract lesions disrupt dead reckoning in the rat , 2011, The European journal of neuroscience.

[57]  David K Bilkey,et al.  Bilateral peripheral vestibular lesions produce long-term changes in spatial learning in the rat. , 2003, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.

[58]  William H Warren,et al.  Path Integration from Optic Flow and Body Senses in a Homing Task , 2002, Perception.

[59]  T. Hafting,et al.  Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex , 2005, Nature.

[60]  A. Etienne,et al.  Limitations in the Assessment of Path Dependent Information , 1988 .

[61]  R. Morris,et al.  Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions , 1982, Nature.

[62]  Jeffrey S. Taube,et al.  Origins of landmark encoding in the brain , 2011, Trends in Neurosciences.

[63]  J S Taube,et al.  Preferential use of the landmark navigational system by head direction cells in rats. , 1995, Behavioral neuroscience.

[64]  Simon Benhamou,et al.  Spatial memory in large scale movements: Efficiency and limitation of the egocentric coding process , 1990 .

[65]  Jeffrey S. Taube,et al.  The vestibular contribution to the head direction signal and navigation , 2014, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[66]  Jeffrey S. Taube,et al.  Path integration: how the head direction signal maintains and corrects spatial orientation , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[67]  Jingyi Ma,et al.  Activation of immobility‐related hippocampal theta by cholinergic septohippocampal neurons during vestibular stimulation , 2012, Hippocampus.

[68]  J. O'Keefe,et al.  The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. , 1971, Brain research.