Neural Correlates of Optimal Multisensory Decision Making under Time-Varying Reliabilities with an Invariant Linear Probabilistic Population Code

Perceptual decisions are often based on multiple sensory inputs whose reliabilities rapidly vary over time, yet little is known about how our brain integrates these inputs to optimize behavior. Here we show multisensory evidence with time-varying reliability can be accumulated near optimally, in a Bayesian sense, by simply taking time-invariant linear combinations of neural activity across time and modalities, as long as the neural code for the sensory inputs is close to an invariant linear probabilistic population code (ilPPC). Recordings in the lateral intraparietal area (LIP) while macaques optimally performed a vestibular-visual multisensory decision-making task revealed that LIP population activity reflects an integration process consistent with the ilPPC theory. Moreover, LIP accumulates momentary evidence proportional to vestibular acceleration and visual velocity which are encoded in sensory areas with a close approximation to ilPPCs. Together, these results provide a remarkably simple and biologically plausible solution to optimal multisensory decision making.

[1]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. , 2006, Psychological review.

[2]  Wei Ji Ma,et al.  Bayesian inference with probabilistic population codes , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  Yong Gu,et al.  Spatiotemporal properties of vestibular responses in area MSTd. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  Alexandre Pouget,et al.  Optimal multisensory decision-making in a reaction-time task , 2014, eLife.

[5]  M N Shadlen,et al.  Motion perception: seeing and deciding. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[6]  Yong Gu,et al.  Decoding of MSTd Population Activity Accounts for Variations in the Precision of Heading Perception , 2010, Neuron.

[7]  Constant D. Beugré,et al.  The neural basis of decision making , 2018 .

[8]  Yong Gu,et al.  Causal Links between Dorsal Medial Superior Temporal Area Neurons and Multisensory Heading Perception , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  Matthew T. Kaufman,et al.  A category-free neural population supports evolving demands during decision-making , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  Dora E Angelaki,et al.  Transformation of spatiotemporal dynamics in the macaque vestibular system from otolith afferents to cortex , 2017, eLife.

[11]  A. Pouget,et al.  Variance as a Signature of Neural Computations during Decision Making , 2011, Neuron.

[12]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. , 2004, Psychological review.

[13]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Pathways for motion analysis: Cortical connections of the medial superior temporal and fundus of the superior temporal visual areas in the macaque , 1990, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[14]  Chandramouli Chandrasekaran,et al.  Computational principles and models of multisensory integration , 2017, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[15]  Jennifer K Bizley,et al.  Where are multisensory signals combined for perceptual decision-making? , 2016, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[16]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  Distributed Visual–Vestibular Processing in the Cerebral Cortex of Man and Macaque , 2017 .

[17]  A. Pouget,et al.  Reading population codes: a neural implementation of ideal observers , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  H Sompolinsky,et al.  Simple models for reading neuronal population codes. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .

[20]  A. Pouget,et al.  Efficient computation and cue integration with noisy population codes , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  Representation of Vestibular and Visual Cues to Self-Motion in Ventral Intraparietal Cortex , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  A. Pouget,et al.  Information-limiting correlations , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  Christopher R Fetsch,et al.  Neural correlates of reliability-based cue weighting during multisensory integration , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[24]  Yong Gu,et al.  Evidence for a Causal Contribution of Macaque Vestibular, But Not Intraparietal, Cortex to Heading Perception , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  Donald Laming,et al.  Information theory of choice-reaction times , 1968 .

[26]  W. Newsome,et al.  Context-dependent computation by recurrent dynamics in prefrontal cortex , 2013, Nature.

[27]  Timothy D. Hanks,et al.  Probabilistic Population Codes for Bayesian Decision Making , 2008, Neuron.

[28]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  The Diffusion Decision Model: Theory and Data for Two-Choice Decision Tasks , 2008, Neural Computation.

[29]  Mathew E. Diamond,et al.  Supralinear and Supramodal Integration of Visual and Tactile Signals in Rats: Psychophysics and Neuronal Mechanisms , 2018, Neuron.

[30]  Jacob L Yates,et al.  The Role of the Lateral Intraparietal Area in (the Study of) Decision Making. , 2017, Annual review of neuroscience.

[31]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Response of Neurons in the Lateral Intraparietal Area during a Combined Visual Discrimination Reaction Time Task , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  Timothy D. Hanks,et al.  Bounded Integration in Parietal Cortex Underlies Decisions Even When Viewing Duration Is Dictated by the Environment , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  Xiao-Jing Wang,et al.  Probabilistic Decision Making by Slow Reverberation in Cortical Circuits , 2002, Neuron.

[34]  Jay A. Hennig,et al.  Signal Multiplexing and Single-Neuron Computations in Lateral Intraparietal Area During Decision-Making , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  Yong Gu Vestibular signals in primate cortex for self-motion perception , 2018, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[36]  Jonathan W. Pillow,et al.  Dissociated functional significance of decision-related activity in the primate dorsal stream , 2016, Nature.

[37]  Alexandre Pouget,et al.  Insights from a Simple Expression for Linear Fisher Information in a Recurrently Connected Population of Spiking Neurons , 2011, Neural Computation.

[38]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Modeling Response Times for Two-Choice Decisions , 1998 .

[39]  Sen Song,et al.  Highly Nonrandom Features of Synaptic Connectivity in Local Cortical Circuits , 2005, PLoS biology.

[40]  Xiao-Jing Wang,et al.  A Recurrent Network Mechanism of Time Integration in Perceptual Decisions , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  R. Andersen,et al.  Saccade-related activity in the lateral intraparietal area. I. Temporal properties; comparison with area 7a. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  Xiao-Jing Wang,et al.  Reward-based training of recurrent neural networks for cognitive and value-based tasks , 2016, bioRxiv.

[43]  A. Pouget,et al.  Tuning curve sharpening for orientation selectivity: coding efficiency and the impact of correlations , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[44]  Dora E Angelaki,et al.  Functional Specializations of the Ventral Intraparietal Area for Multisensory Heading Discrimination , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  Il Memming Park,et al.  Encoding and decoding in parietal cortex during sensorimotor decision-making , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[46]  J. Gold,et al.  The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[47]  Gregory C. DeAngelis,et al.  A Comparison of Vestibular Spatiotemporal Tuning in Macaque Parietoinsular Vestibular Cortex, Ventral Intraparietal Area, and Medial Superior Temporal Area , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  Gregory C. DeAngelis,et al.  Bridging the gap between theories of sensory cue integration and the physiology of multisensory neurons , 2013, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[49]  Carlos D. Brody,et al.  Fronto-parietal Cortical Circuits Encode Accumulated Evidence with a Diversity of Timescales , 2017, Neuron.

[50]  W. Newsome,et al.  Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[51]  W. Richards,et al.  Perception as Bayesian Inference , 2008 .

[52]  Maninder K. Kahlon,et al.  Visual Motion Analysis for Pursuit Eye Movements in Area MT of Macaque Monkeys , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  Dora E Angelaki,et al.  Visual and Nonvisual Contributions to Three-Dimensional Heading Selectivity in the Medial Superior Temporal Area , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  Neural correlates of multisensory cue integration in macaque MSTd , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.