Motor resources in peripersonal space are intrinsic to spatial encoding: evidence from motor interference.

The aim of this study was to explore the role of motor resources in peripersonal space encoding: are they intrinsic to spatial processes or due to action potentiality of objects? To answer this question, we disentangled the effects of motor resources on object manipulability and spatial processing in peripersonal and extrapersonal spaces. Participants had to localize manipulable and non-manipulable 3-D stimuli presented within peripersonal or extrapersonal spaces of an immersive virtual reality scenario. To assess the contribution of motor resources to the spatial task a motor interference paradigm was used. In Experiment 1, localization judgments were provided with the left hand while the right dominant arm could be free or blocked. Results showed that participants were faster and more accurate in localizing both manipulable and non-manipulable stimuli in peripersonal space with their arms free. On the other hand, in extrapersonal space there was no significant effect of motor interference. Experiment 2 replicated these results by using alternatively both hands to give the response and controlling the possible effect of the orientation of object handles. Overall, the pattern of results suggests that the encoding of peripersonal space involves motor processes per se, and not because of the presence of manipulable stimuli. It is argued that this motor grounding reflects the adaptive need of anticipating what may happen near the body and preparing to react in time.

[1]  Sarah H. Creem-Regehr,et al.  Neural representations of graspable objects: are tools special? , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[2]  R. Ellis,et al.  Action priming by briefly presented objects. , 2004, Acta psychologica.

[3]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  B. Rossion,et al.  Revisiting Snodgrass and Vanderwart's Object Pictorial Set: The Role of Surface Detail in Basic-Level Object Recognition , 2004, Perception.

[5]  Charles G. Gross,et al.  REVIEW ■ : Multiple Representations of Space in the Brain , 1995 .

[6]  W. R. Brain VISUAL DISORIENTATION WITH SPECIAL REFERENCE TO LESIONS OF THE RIGHT CEREBRAL HEMISPHERE , 1941 .

[7]  T. Ziemke,et al.  Theories and computational models of affordance and mirror systems: An integrative review , 2013, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[8]  Alan Cowey,et al.  Spatial neglect in near and far space investigated by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Space coding by premotor cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  V. Gallese Embodied simulation: From neurons to phenomenal experience , 2005 .

[11]  Dylan F. Cooke,et al.  Parieto-frontal interactions, personal space, and defensive behavior , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  C. Gross,et al.  Coding of visual space by premotor neurons. , 1994, Science.

[13]  E. Hall,et al.  The Hidden Dimension , 1970 .

[14]  Matthew Heath,et al.  Visuomotor system uses target features unavailable to conscious awareness , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[15]  A. Borghi,et al.  Embodied cognition and beyond: Acting and sensing the body , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  G Rizzolatti,et al.  The Space Around Us , 1997, Science.

[17]  Elisabeth Pacherie,et al.  The Sense of Control and the Sense of Agency , 2007 .

[18]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Two different streams form the dorsal visual system: anatomy and functions , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  G. Olivier,et al.  Visual objects can potentiate a grasping neural simulation which interferes with manual response execution. , 2009, Acta psychologica.

[20]  Anna M. Borghi,et al.  Words as Social Tools: An Embodied View on Abstract Concepts , 2014 .

[21]  Claudio Brozzoli,et al.  Peripersonal space : a multisensory interface for body-objects interactions , 2009 .

[22]  R. Hari,et al.  Temporal dynamics of cortical representation for action. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[23]  V. Gallese,et al.  Objects and their nouns in peripersonal space , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  P. Derambure,et al.  Perceiving What Is Reachable Depends on Motor Representations: Evidence from a Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Study , 2008, PloS one.

[25]  Marc H. Schieber,et al.  Inactivation of the ventral premotor cortex biases the laterality of motoric choices , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Mirror Neurons Differentially Encode the Peripersonal and Extrapersonal Space of Monkeys , 2009, Science.

[27]  L. Barsalou,et al.  Whither structured representation? , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[28]  Ed Symes,et al.  Dissociating object-based and space-based affordances , 2005 .

[29]  Leonardo Fogassi,et al.  Motor functions of the parietal lobe , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[30]  G. Humphreys Objects, affordances ... action! , 2001 .

[31]  R. Logie Visuo-spatial Working Memory , 1994 .

[32]  Tina Iachini,et al.  Embodied perception of reachable space: how do we manage threatening objects? , 2012, Cognitive Processing.

[33]  Alex Martin,et al.  Representation of Manipulable Man-Made Objects in the Dorsal Stream , 2000, NeuroImage.

[34]  A. Berti,et al.  When Far Becomes Near: Remapping of Space by Tool Use , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[35]  Alice Mado Proverbio,et al.  250ms to code for action affordance during observation of manipulable objects , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  Luigi Trojano,et al.  Lateralization of egocentric and allocentric spatial processing after parietal brain lesions , 2009, Brain and Cognition.

[37]  Albert Postma,et al.  Frames of reference and categorical and coordinate spatial relations: a hierarchical organisation , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980 .

[39]  A. Sirigu,et al.  Motor and Visual Imagery as Two Complementary but Neurally Dissociable Mental Processes , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[40]  A Bartolo,et al.  Neurobiological basis of reachability judgement: an fMRI study , 2009, NeuroImage.

[41]  Mel Slater,et al.  Place illusion and plausibility can lead to realistic behaviour in immersive virtual environments , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[42]  Ineke J M van der Ham,et al.  The relationship between allocentric and egocentric frames of reference and categorical and coordinate spatial information processing , 2011, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[43]  Fred H. Previc,et al.  The Neuropsychology of 3-D Space , 1998 .

[44]  R. Ellis,et al.  The potentiation of grasp types during visual object categorization , 2001 .

[45]  Alessandro Farnè,et al.  Neuropsychological evidence of modular organization of the near peripersonal space , 2005, Neurology.

[46]  J. Loomis,et al.  Interpersonal Distance in Immersive Virtual Environments , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[47]  S. Anderson,et al.  Attentional processes link perception and action , 2002, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[48]  Nicholas P. Holmes,et al.  The body schema and multisensory representation(s) of peripersonal space , 2004, Cognitive Processing.

[49]  Lawrence W Barsalou,et al.  Simulation, situated conceptualization, and prediction , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[50]  M. Sereno,et al.  Mapping multisensory parietal face and body areas in humans , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[51]  V. Gallese The "Conscious" Dorsal Stream: Embodied Simulation and its Role in Space and Action Conscious Awareness , 2007 .

[52]  T Brochier,et al.  Simultaneous recording of macaque premotor and primary motor cortex neuronal populations reveals different functional contributions to visuomotor grasp. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[53]  R. Ward,et al.  S-R correspondence effects of irrelevant visual affordance: Time course and specificity of response activation , 2002 .

[54]  M. Arbib,et al.  Grasping objects: the cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation , 1995, Trends in Neurosciences.

[55]  B. Hommel Event files: feature binding in and across perception and action , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[56]  Sandro Rubichi,et al.  Are visual stimuli sufficient to evoke motor information? Studies with hand primes , 2007, Neuroscience Letters.

[57]  J. A. Stevens,et al.  Interference effects demonstrate distinct roles for visual and motor imagery during the mental representation of human action , 2005, Cognition.

[58]  Roberto Nicoletti,et al.  Categorization and action: what about object consistence? , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[59]  G. Vingerhoets,et al.  Conceptual and physical object qualities contribute differently to motor affordances , 2009, Brain and Cognition.

[60]  Dylan F. Cooke,et al.  Sensorimotor integration in the precentral gyrus: polysensory neurons and defensive movements. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[61]  Walter C. Gogel,et al.  An indirect method of measuring perceived distance from familiar size , 1976 .

[62]  Yann Coello,et al.  Motor representations and the perception of space: perceptual judgments of the boundary of action space , 2010 .

[63]  Anna M. Borghi,et al.  Categorization and sensorimotor interaction with objects , 2008, Brain and Cognition.

[64]  M. Costantini,et al.  The space of affordances: A TMS study , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[65]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Embodied Language: A Review of the Role of the Motor System in Language Comprehension , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[66]  R. Ellis,et al.  Micro-affordance: the potentiation of components of action by seen objects. , 2000, British journal of psychology.

[67]  Yann Coello,et al.  Embodiment, spatial categorisation and action , 2007, Consciousness and Cognition.

[68]  Massimiliano Masullo,et al.  Multisensory Assessment of Acoustic Comfort Aboard Metros: a Virtual Reality Study , 2012 .

[69]  Alessandro Farnè,et al.  Peripersonal Space and Body Schema: Two Labels for the Same Concept? , 2009, Brain Topography.

[70]  R. Ellis,et al.  On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[71]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[72]  B. Stein,et al.  The Merging of the Senses , 1993 .

[73]  Massimiliano Masullo,et al.  Immersive virtual reality and environmental noise assessment: An innovative audio-visual approach , 2013 .

[74]  H. Ehrsson,et al.  Behavioural Brain Research , 1999 .

[75]  Gaetano Tieri,et al.  Where does an object trigger an action? An investigation about affordances in space , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[76]  Ed Symes,et al.  Visual object affordances: object orientation. , 2007, Acta psychologica.

[77]  K. Zilles,et al.  Neural consequences of acting in near versus far space: a physiological basis for clinical dissociations. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[78]  Jérémy Bourgeois,et al.  Effect of visuomotor calibration and uncertainty on the perception of peripersonal space , 2012, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[79]  Joshua P. Salmon,et al.  Norms for two types of manipulability (graspability and functional usage), familiarity, and age of acquisition for 320 photographs of objects , 2010, Behavior research methods.

[80]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .