Read Montaguel and The Predictive Brain : Temporal

Introduction Some forms of synaptic plasticity depend o n the temporal coincidence of presynaptic activity and postsynaptic response. This requirement is consistent with the Hebbian, o r correlational, type of learning rule used in many neural network models. Recent evidence suggests that synaptic plasticity may depend in part o n the production of a membrane permeant-diffusible signal so that spatial volume may also be involved in correlational learning rules. This latter form of synaptic change has been called volume learning. In both Hebbian and volume learning rules, interaction among synaptic inputs depends o n the degree of coincidence of the inputs and is otherwise insensitive to their exact temporal order. Conditioning experiments and psychophysical studies have shown, however, that most animals are highly sensitive to the temporal order of the sensory inputs. Although these experiments assay the behavior of the entire animal o r perceptual system, they raise the possibility that nervous systems may be sensitive to temporally ordered events at many spatial and temporal scales. We suggest here the existence of a new class of learning rule, called apredictiue Hebbian learning rule, that is sensitive to the temporal ordering of synaptic inputs. We show how this predictive learning rule could act at single synaptic connections and through diffuse neuromodulatory systems. Most biologically feasible theories of how experience-dependent changes take place in real neuronal networks use some variant of the notion that the efficacy or "strength of a synaptic connection from one cell to another can be modified on the basis of its history. In this theoretical work it is generally assumed that modifications of synaptic eEcacy, by acting over a large population of synapses, can account for interesting forms of learning and memory. This theoretical assumption prevails primarily because of its intuitive appeal, its accessibility to analysis, some provocative relations to biological data, and a lack of good alternatives. Recent work demonstrates that simple abstract learning algorithms, if given appropriately coded input, can produce complicated mappings from input to output. These efforts include networks that learn to pronounce written text (Sejnowski and Rosenberg 1987), play master level backgammon (Tesauro 1994), and recognize handwritten characters (Le Cun et al. 1990). As pointed out by Crick (1989) and others, many of these efforts are not good models of the vertebrate brain; however, they can be quite valuable for identrfying the informational requirements LEARNING & MEMORY 1 :1-33 O 1994 by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press ISSN1072-0502194 $5.00 L E A R N I N G M E M O R Y Montague and Sejnowski involved in specific tasks. Moreover, they point out some of the computational constraints to which brains are subject. An awareness of the computational constraints involved in a particular problem can guide theories that explain how real brains are constructed (Churchlanci and Sejnowski 1992). Although abstract networks have provided some insight into the top-down constraints that nervous systems face, these approaches are of limited use in gaining insight into how various problems have been solved by real brains. For example, the actual learning mechanisms that are used in biological systems also satisfy additional constraints that arise from the known properties of neurons and synapses. In this paper we focus on learning rules that are supported by biological data and consider the strengths and weaknesses of these rules by measuring them against both computational and biological constraints. Taking this dual approach, we show that computational concerns applicable to the behavior and survival of the animal can work hand in hand with biologically feasible synaptic mechanisms to explain and predict experimental data. Correlational Theoretical accounts of how neural activity actually changes synaptic Learning function typically rely on a local correlational learning rule to model Rules-Learning synaptic plasticity. A correlational learning rule, often called a Hebbian Driven by Temporal learning rule, uses the correlation between presynaptic activity and Coincidence postsynaptic response to drive changes in synaptic efficacy (Fig. 1) (Hertz et al. 1991; Churchland and Sejnowski 1992). One simple expression of a Hebbian learning rule is where, at time t, w(t) is a connection strength (weight), x ( t ) is a measure of presynaptic activity, y(t) is a measure of postsynaptic activity (e.g., firing rate or probability of firing), and q is a fuced learning rate. This kind of learning rule is called local because the signals sufficient for changing synaptic eff~cacy are assumed to be generated locally at each synaptic contact. One form of this learning rule was initially proposed by Donald Hebb in 1949 (Hebb 1949). Subsequent theoretical and computational efforts have exploited Hebb's idea and used correlational learning rules to account successfully for aspects of map formation and self-organization of visual and somatosensory cortex (von der Malsburg 1973; von der Malsburg and Willshaw 1977; Montague et al. 199 1). For example, various computational schemes employing Hebbian learning rules have accounted for the formation of cortical receptive fields (Bienenstock et al. 1982; Linsker 1986, 1988), ocular dominance columns (Miller et al. 1989), orientation maps (von der Malsburg 1973; Obermayer et al. 1990; Miller 1994), dii-ectional selectivity (Sereno and Sereno 1991), and disparity tuning (Berns et al. 1993). Correlational learning rules also provide a reasonable theoretical framework for synaptic plasticity observed in the hippocampus (Kelso et al. 1986; Bliss and Lynch 1988), cerebellum (Ito 1986, 1989), and neocortex (Kirkwood et al. 1993). Below, we review some of the biological evidence from the vertebrate nervous system that supports this simple learning rule as a descriptor of synaptic change during both activity-dependent development and synaptic modification in the adult. We subsequently suggest that changes L E A R N I N G M E M O R Y THE PREDICTIVE BRAIN Figure 1: Hebbian Learning. (A) Inputs xi provide excitatory drive to a neuron through connection strengths or "weights". Inputs x, and x, are sufficiently correlated to permit cooperation along a section of dendrite (shaded area) through voltage or second messengers. Through an expression like equation 1, the weights of these connections will be increased. Input x, is not active during this coincident activation of x, and x,. The weight of x,'s connection could be decreased by a depression rule that depressed all synaptic contacts that were not sufficiently correlated with the postsynaptic response (shaded area). Without such a rule, weights can grow without bound. To prevent this, a homeostatic constraint that limits the total synaptic strength supported by the recipient neuron is typically used. This i s just one possible way to normalize the weights. The issue of how and why normalization is biologically reasonable is critical.' Normalization can give stability to the Hebb rule, but, depending on its implementation, it can cause the weight vector to converge to different values. In the presence of additional constraints (see text) for the learning rule, a Hebb rule will extract the principal component from the correlations in the input patterns that occur and the vector of weights wil l come to point in the direction of the first principle component of the "data" generated by the input activities (Oja 1982). The pattern of weights that develops can be analyzed in terms of the covariance matrix of the input activities (see text). (B ) Graph of input activity along two inputs, x, and x, . Each point i s a pair of activity levels for the two inputs in (A). The inputs cluster along a straight line, indicating a strong correlation. The approximate direction of the principal component i s along this line. in this description are required by both experimental and theoretical work. The primary change is predicated on the need for brain mechanisms sensitive to temporally ordered input, a problem that has most likely been solved by brains across a range of spatiotemporal scales. We marshal arguments and review detailed evidence in support of this suggestion and point out those aspects of the proposed changes that are important for understanding learning and memory in the vertebrate brain. Developmental In the vertebrate nervous system, afferent axons find their appropriate Evidence for Hebbian target structures through interactions with local environmental cues and Learning Rules target-derived information (Bonhoeffer and Huff 1985; Dodd and Jesse1 1988; Stuermer 1988; Harris 1989; Heffner et al. 1990; O'Leary et al. 1990; Placzek et al. 1990; Stretevan 1990). After reaching target structures, there is strong evidence that activity-dependent processes are critical in determining the development of mappings between peripheral sensory structures and their more centrally located target structures, including the optic tectum, thalamus, and cerebral cortex (Hubel and Wiesel 1965, 1970; Hubel et al. 1977; Meyer 1982; Stryker and Harris 1986; Stretevan et al. 1988). Specific mappings arise in these targets because temporal contiguity in axonal firing is somehow translated into L E A R N I N G M E M O R Y Montague and Sejnowski spatial contiguity of synaptic contacts. Hence, activity-dependent processes are involved at least with the initial self-otganizalion of mappings in the tectum, thalamus, and cortex. After normal developmental periods, activiw-dependent processes are also involved in the reorganization of sensory mappings in the adult. For example, the adult cerebral cortex has been shown to be surprisingly plastic (for review, see Kaas 1991; Merzenich and Sameshima 1993) following changes to the environment such as retinal damage (Kaas et al. 1990; Gilbert and W

[1]  J L McGaugh,et al.  The role of interactions between the cholinergic system and other neuromodulatory systems in learing and memory , 1991, Synapse.

[2]  R. F. Thompson,et al.  Modeling the neural substrates of associative learning and memory: a computational approach. , 1987, Psychological review.

[3]  Joseph Loscalzo,et al.  A redox-based mechanism for the neuroprotective and neurodestructive effects of nitric oxide and related nitroso-compounds , 1993, Nature.

[4]  A. Damasio,et al.  Knowledge without awareness: an autonomic index of facial recognition by prosopagnosics. , 1985, Science.

[5]  S. Foote,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence for the involvement of the locus coeruleus in alerting, orienting, and attending. , 1991, Progress in brain research.

[6]  W. Singer,et al.  The formation of cooperative cell assemblies in the visual cortex. , 1990, The Journal of experimental biology.

[7]  R. Linsker,et al.  From basic network principles to neural architecture , 1986 .

[8]  S. Lipton,et al.  Effect of nitric oxide production on the redox modulatory site of the NMDA receptor-channel complex , 1992, Neuron.

[9]  Ralph Linsker,et al.  Self-organization in a perceptual network , 1988, Computer.

[10]  D. Hubel,et al.  The period of susceptibility to the physiological effects of unilateral eye closure in kittens , 1970, The Journal of physiology.

[11]  P. Dayan,et al.  Volume Learning: Signaling Covariance Through Neural Tissue , 1993 .

[12]  R L Meyer,et al.  Tetrodotoxin blocks the formation of ocular dominance columns in goldfish. , 1982, Science.

[13]  T. Jessell,et al.  Axon guidance and the patterning of neuronal projections in vertebrates. , 1988, Science.

[14]  Gerald Tesauro,et al.  TD-Gammon, a Self-Teaching Backgammon Program, Achieves Master-Level Play , 1994, Neural Computation.

[15]  W Singer,et al.  Disruption of experience-dependent synaptic modifications in striate cortex by infusion of an NMDA receptor antagonist , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[16]  M. Sur,et al.  Prenatal disruption of binocular interactions creates novel lamination in the cat's lateral geniculate nucleus , 1988, Visual Neuroscience.

[17]  Allen I. Selverston,et al.  Model Neural Networks and Behavior , 1985, Springer US.

[18]  S. Lea,et al.  Contemporary Animal Learning Theory, Anthony Dickinson. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1981), xii, +177 pp. £12.50 hardback, £3.95 paperback , 1981 .

[19]  T. Bliss,et al.  A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus , 1993, Nature.

[20]  S. Deadwyler,et al.  Long-term potentiation : from biophysics to behavior , 1988 .

[21]  J. Kaas,et al.  Dynamic features of sensory and motor maps , 1992, Current Biology.

[22]  Margaret E. Sereno,et al.  Learning to See Rotation and Dilation with a Hebb Rule , 1990, NIPS.

[23]  M. Castro-Alamancos,et al.  Facilitation and recovery of shuttle box avoidance behavior after frontal cortex lesions in induced by a contingent electrical stimulation in the ventral tegmental nucleus , 1992, Behavioural Brain Research.

[24]  Y. Prigent [Long term depression]. , 1989, Annales medico-psychologiques.

[25]  E. Capaldi,et al.  The organization of behavior. , 1992, Journal of applied behavior analysis.

[26]  E. Oja Simplified neuron model as a principal component analyzer , 1982, Journal of mathematical biology.

[27]  C. Stevens,et al.  Presynaptic mechanism for long-term potentiation in the hippocampus , 1990, Nature.

[28]  G. Böhme,et al.  A Role for Nitric Oxide in Long‐term Potentiation , 1992, The European journal of neuroscience.

[29]  D. O'Leary,et al.  Target selection by cortical axons: alternative mechanisms to establish axonal connections in the developing brain. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[30]  J. Kaas Plasticity of sensory and motor maps in adult mammals. , 1991, Annual review of neuroscience.

[31]  R. Rescorla A theory of pavlovian conditioning: The effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement , 1972 .

[32]  E. Kandel,et al.  Is there a cell-biological alphabet for simple forms of learning? , 1984, Psychological review.

[33]  Charles F. Stevens,et al.  Reversal of long-term potentiation by inhibitors of haem oxygenase , 1993, Nature.

[34]  W. Singer,et al.  Modulation of visual cortical plasticity by acetylcholine and noradrenaline , 1986, Nature.

[35]  G. Stent A physiological mechanism for Hebb's postulate of learning. , 1973, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[36]  M. Stryker,et al.  Binocular impulse blockade prevents the formation of ocular dominance columns in cat visual cortex , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[37]  A R Damasio,et al.  Amnesia following basal forebrain lesions. , 1985, Archives of neurology.

[38]  Bradley L. Schlaggar,et al.  Postsynaptic control of plasticity in developing somatosensory cortex , 1993, Nature.

[39]  W. Singer,et al.  Changes in the circuitry of the kitten visual cortex are gated by postsynaptic activity , 1979, Nature.

[40]  H. Ritter,et al.  A principle for the formation of the spatial structure of cortical feature maps. , 1990, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[41]  G. Edelman,et al.  The NO hypothesis: possible effects of a short-lived, rapidly diffusible signal in the development and function of the nervous system. , 1990, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[42]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Forward Models: Supervised Learning with a Distal Teacher , 1992, Cogn. Sci..

[43]  W. Schultz,et al.  Responses of monkey dopamine neurons during learning of behavioral reactions. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  E. Kandel,et al.  Nitric oxide and carbon monoxide produce activity-dependent long-term synaptic enhancement in hippocampus. , 1993, Science.

[45]  A G Barto,et al.  Toward a modern theory of adaptive networks: expectation and prediction. , 1981, Psychological review.

[46]  G. Edelman,et al.  Spatial signaling in the development and function of neural connections. , 1991, Cerebral cortex.

[47]  K. Jellinger,et al.  Brain dopamine and the syndromes of Parkinson and Huntington. Clinical, morphological and neurochemical correlations. , 1973, Journal of the neurological sciences.

[48]  T. Robbins,et al.  Forebrain norepinephrine: role in controlled information processing in the rat. , 1992, Neuropsychopharmacology : official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology.

[49]  Francis Crick,et al.  The recent excitement about neural networks , 1989, Nature.

[50]  J. Rauschecker,et al.  Mechanisms of visual plasticity: Hebb synapses, NMDA receptors, and beyond. , 1991, Physiological reviews.

[51]  G. Recanzone,et al.  Adaptive mechanisms in cortical networks underlying cortical contributions to learning and nondeclarative memory. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[52]  M. Stryker,et al.  Neural plasticity without postsynaptic action potentials: less-active inputs become dominant when kitten visual cortical cells are pharmacologically inhibited. , 1988, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[53]  Anders Krogh,et al.  Introduction to the theory of neural computation , 1994, The advanced book program.

[54]  P. Redfern Neuromuscular transmission in new‐born rats , 1970, The Journal of physiology.

[55]  J. Bolz,et al.  Non-Hebbian synapses in rat visual cortex. , 1990, Neuroreport.

[56]  M. Bear,et al.  Homosynaptic long-term depression in area CA1 of hippocampus and effects of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor blockade. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[57]  Terence D. Sanger,et al.  Optimal unsupervised learning in a single-layer linear feedforward neural network , 1989, Neural Networks.

[58]  B. C. Motter,et al.  The influence of attentive fixation upon the excitability of the light- sensitive neurons of the posterior parietal cortex , 1981, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[59]  M. Stryker,et al.  Prenatal tetrodotoxin infusion blocks segregation of retinogeniculate afferents. , 1988, Science.

[60]  K. Miller,et al.  Ocular dominance column development: analysis and simulation. , 1989, Science.

[61]  P. Klatt,et al.  Brain nitric oxide synthase is a haemoprotein. , 1992, The Biochemical journal.

[62]  S. Kelso,et al.  Hebbian synapses in hippocampus. , 1986, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[63]  D. Hubel,et al.  Binocular interaction in striate cortex of kittens reared with artificial squint. , 1965, Journal of neurophysiology.

[64]  C. Malsburg,et al.  How to label nerve cells so that they can interconnect in an ordered fashion. , 1977, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[65]  C A Stuermer,et al.  Retinotopic organization of the developing retinotectal projection in the zebrafish embryo , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[66]  Solomon H. Snyder,et al.  Nitric oxide, a novel neuronal messenger , 1992, Neuron.

[67]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Homosynaptic long-term depression in hippocampus and mescsrtex , 1990 .

[68]  M. Constantine-Paton,et al.  N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonists disrupt the formation of a mammalian neural map. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[69]  P Dayan,et al.  Expectation learning in the brain using diffuse ascending projections , 1992 .

[70]  C. H. Bailey,et al.  The anatomy of a memory: convergence of results across a diversity of tests , 1988, Trends in Neurosciences.

[71]  Lawrence D. Jackel,et al.  Handwritten Digit Recognition with a Back-Propagation Network , 1989, NIPS.

[72]  D J Willshaw,et al.  A marker induction mechanism for the establishment of ordered neural mappings: its application to the retinotectal problem. , 1979, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[73]  Tim V. P. Bliss,et al.  The search for retrograde messengers in long-term potentiation , 1993 .

[74]  L. Real,et al.  Why are Bumble Bees Risk Averse , 1987 .

[75]  C. Zorumski,et al.  Nitric oxide inhibitors facilitate the induction of hippocampal long-term potentiation by modulating NMDA responses. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[76]  H. Wigström,et al.  On long-lasting potentiation in the hippocampus: a proposed mechanism for its dependence on coincident pre- and postsynaptic activity. , 1985, Acta physiologica Scandinavica.

[77]  D. O'Leary,et al.  Development of projection neuron types, axon pathways, and patterned connections of the mammalian cortex , 1993, Neuron.

[78]  N. Swindale A model for the formation of ocular dominance stripes , 1980, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[79]  M. Pettet,et al.  Dynamic changes in receptive-field size in cat primary visual cortex. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[80]  Graham L. Collingridge,et al.  Temporally distinct pre- and post-synaptic mechanisms maintain long-term potentiation , 1989, Nature.

[81]  J. Kaas,et al.  Reorganization of retinotopic cortical maps in adult mammals after lesions of the retina. , 1990, Science.

[82]  D. Clifford,et al.  Inhibition of long-term potentiation by NMDA-mediated nitric oxide release. , 1992, Science.

[83]  E. Bienenstock,et al.  Theory for the development of neuron selectivity: orientation specificity and binocular interaction in visual cortex , 1982, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[84]  M. Merzenich,et al.  Cortical plasticity and memory , 1993, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[85]  D. Willshaw The establishment and the subsequent elimination of polyneuronal innervation of developing muscle: theoretical considerations , 1981, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[86]  R. Tsien,et al.  Presynaptic enhancement shown by whole-cell recordings of long-term potentiation in hippocampal slices , 1990, Nature.

[87]  KD Miller A model for the development of simple cell receptive fields and the ordered arrangement of orientation columns through activity-dependent competition between ON- and OFF-center inputs , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[88]  Richard S. Sutton,et al.  Time-Derivative Models of Pavlovian Reinforcement , 1990 .

[89]  S. Udin,et al.  N-methyl-D-aspartate antagonists prevent interaction of binocular maps in Xenopus tectum , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[90]  Peter Dayan,et al.  Optimal Plasticity from Matrix Memories: What Goes Up Must Come Down , 1990, Neural Computation.

[91]  R. Nicoll,et al.  The current excitement in long term potentiation , 1988, Neuron.

[92]  M. Cynader,et al.  Somatosensory cortical map changes following digit amputation in adult monkeys , 1984, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[93]  Irwin J. Kopin,et al.  The Biochemical Basis of Neuropharmacology , 1971, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.

[94]  R. Sutton,et al.  Simulation of the classically conditioned nictitating membrane response by a neuron-like adaptive element: Response topography, neuronal firing, and interstimulus intervals , 1986, Behavioural Brain Research.

[95]  W. Harris Local positional cues in the neuroepithelium guide retinal axons in embryonic Xenopus brain , 1989, Nature.

[96]  C. Malsburg,et al.  How patterned neural connections can be set up by self-organization , 1976, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[97]  C. Iadecola,et al.  Regulation of the cerebral microcirculation during neural activity: is nitric oxide the missing link? , 1993, Trends in Neurosciences.

[98]  M. Bear,et al.  Common forms of synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus and neocortex in vitro. , 1993, Science.

[99]  Michael P. Stryker,et al.  Modification of retinal ganglion cell axon morphology by prenatal infusion of tetrodotoxin , 1988, Nature.

[100]  R. Malenka,et al.  The influence of prior synaptic activity on the induction of long-term potentiation. , 1992, Science.

[101]  G M Edelman,et al.  Selective networks capable of representative transformations, limited generalizations, and associative memory. , 1982, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[102]  L E Mays,et al.  Signal transformations required for the generation of saccadic eye movements. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[103]  A. Aertsen,et al.  Synaptic plasticity in rat hippocampal slice cultures: local "Hebbian" conjunction of pre- and postsynaptic stimulation leads to distributed synaptic enhancement. , 1989, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[104]  Masao Ito Long-term depression as a memory process in the cerebellum , 1986, Neuroscience Research.

[105]  R. Wise Neuroleptics and operant behavior: The anhedonia hypothesis , 1982, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[106]  R. Malenka,et al.  Mechanisms underlying induction of homosynaptic long-term depression in area CA1 of the hippocampus , 1992, Neuron.

[107]  R. Malinow,et al.  Postsynaptic hyperpolarization during conditioning reversibly blocks induction of long-term potentiation , 1986, Nature.

[108]  G. Böhme,et al.  Possible involvement of nitric oxide in long-term potentiation. , 1991, European journal of pharmacology.

[109]  T. Jessell,et al.  Orientation of commissural axons in vitro in response to a floor plate-derived chemoattractant. , 1990, Development.

[110]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Storing covariance with nonlinearly interacting neurons , 1977, Journal of mathematical biology.

[111]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  Parallel Networks that Learn to Pronounce English Text , 1987, Complex Syst..

[112]  T. Bliss,et al.  Long-term potentiation of the perforant path in vivo is associated with increased glutamate release , 1982, Nature.

[113]  J L McGaugh,et al.  Involvement of hormonal and neuromodulatory systems in the regulation of memory storage. , 1989, Annual review of neuroscience.

[114]  W. Singer,et al.  Blockade of "NMDA" receptors disrupts experience-dependent plasticity of kitten striate cortex. , 1987, Science.

[115]  T. Bliss,et al.  Long‐lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path , 1973, The Journal of physiology.

[116]  D. Madison,et al.  A requirement for the intercellular messenger nitric oxide in long-term potentiation. , 1991, Science.

[117]  W. Schultz Activity of dopamine neurons in the behaving primate , 1992 .

[118]  D. O'Leary,et al.  Target control of collateral extension and directional axon growth in the mammalian brain. , 1990, Science.

[119]  Friedrich Bonhoeffer,et al.  Position-dependent properties of retinal axons and their growth cones , 1985, Nature.

[120]  D. Hubel,et al.  Plasticity of ocular dominance columns in monkey striate cortex. , 1977, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[121]  G M Edelman,et al.  Nitric oxide: linking space and time in the brain. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[122]  L. Kaczmarek,et al.  Neuromodulation : the biochemical control of neuronal excitability , 1987 .

[123]  D. J. Felleman,et al.  Topographic reorganization of somatosensory cortical areas 3b and 1 in adult monkeys following restricted deafferentation , 1983, Neuroscience.

[124]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  A critique of pure vision , 1993 .

[125]  W. Singer,et al.  Different voltage-dependent thresholds for inducing long-term depression and long-term potentiation in slices of rat visual cortex , 1990, Nature.

[126]  D. Sretavan Specific routing of retinal ganglion cell axons at the mammalian optic chiasm during embryonic development , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[127]  L A Real,et al.  Animal choice behavior and the evolution of cognitive architecture , 1991, Science.

[128]  D J Felleman,et al.  Functional reorganization in somatosensory cortical areas 3b and 1 of adult monkeys after median nerve repair: possible relationships to sensory recovery in humans , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[129]  A. Damasio,et al.  Preserved access and processing of social knowledge in a patient with acquired sociopathy due to ventromedial frontal damage , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[130]  M. Merzenich,et al.  Receptive fields in the body-surface map in adult cortex defined by temporally correlated inputs , 1988, Nature.

[131]  R. Nicoll,et al.  An essential role for postsynaptic calmodulin and protein kinase activity in long-term potentiation , 1989, Nature.