How moving objects become animated: The human mirror neuron system assimilates non-biological movement patterns

Abstract The so-called mirror neuron system (MNS) responds when humans observe actions performed by a member of their own species. This activity is understood as an internal motor representation of the observed movement pattern. By contrasting meaningless human hand movements with meaningless artificial movements of objects in space, we tested the claim that exclusively movements belonging to the human motor repertoire have direct access to the MNS. Eighteen participants observed video clips of moving hands and objects while the hemodynamic response was recorded with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Second-level analysis of the hemodynamic response revealed substantially overlapping activation patterns for both types of movements including relevant structures of the MNS (bilateral premotor and parietal areas, occipito-temporal junction, postcentral gyrus and the right superior temporal sulcus). This suggests that perceptual processing of moving hands and objects recruits similar and overlapping cortical networks. Direct comparison of the two movement types revealed stronger activations for hand movements mainly in structures of the MNS suggesting an “expertise effect”. Overall, our results provide evidence that observing movements not explicitly belonging to the human motor repertoire can activate the human MNS, most likely because an association with a biological movement is evoked.

[1]  Nikolaus F. Troje,et al.  Structural encoding and recognition of biological motion: evidence from event-related potentials and source analysis , 2005, Behavioural Brain Research.

[2]  R. E Passingham,et al.  Activations related to “mirror” and “canonical” neurones in the human brain: an fMRI study , 2003, NeuroImage.

[3]  D C Van Essen,et al.  Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  R. Passingham,et al.  Seeing or Doing? Influence of Visual and Motor Familiarity in Action Observation , 2006, Current Biology.

[5]  C. Heyes,et al.  Robotic movement elicits automatic imitation. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[6]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[7]  B. Bertenthal,et al.  Does Perception of Biological Motion Rely on Specific Brain Regions? , 2001, NeuroImage.

[8]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[9]  R. J. Seitz,et al.  A fronto‐parietal circuit for object manipulation in man: evidence from an fMRI‐study , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.

[10]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  T. Allison,et al.  Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  K. Zilles,et al.  Human Somatosensory Area 2: Observer-Independent Cytoarchitectonic Mapping, Interindividual Variability, and Population Map , 2001, NeuroImage.

[13]  Hans-Jochen Heinze,et al.  Shared networks for auditory and motor processing in professional pianists: Evidence from fMRI conjunction , 2006, NeuroImage.

[14]  F. Rösler,et al.  Kinesthetic working memory and action control within the dorsal stream. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Activation in Posterior Superior Temporal Sulcus Parallels Parameter Inducing the Percept of Animacy , 2005, Neuron.

[16]  Philippe A. Chouinard,et al.  Role of the Primary Motor and Dorsal Premotor Cortices in the Anticipation of Forces during Object Lifting , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Intact “biological motion” and “structure from motion” perception in a patient with impaired motion mechanisms: A case study , 1990, Visual Neuroscience.

[18]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[19]  U. Castiello,et al.  The Human Premotor Cortex Is 'Mirror' Only for Biological Actions , 2004, Current Biology.

[20]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Area V5 of the human brain: evidence from a combined study using positron emission tomography and magnetic resonance imaging. , 1993, Cerebral cortex.

[21]  S. Rossi,et al.  Somatosensory processing during movement observation in humans , 2002, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[22]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. , 1995, Psychological review.

[23]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  W. Prinz,et al.  Compatibility between Observed and Executed Finger Movements: Comparing Symbolic, Spatial, and Imitative Cues , 2000, Brain and Cognition.

[25]  Alexander Münchau,et al.  Investigating the human mirror neuron system by means of cortical synchronization during the imitation of biological movements , 2006, NeuroImage.

[26]  Peter Kirsch,et al.  Differential activation of pre-SMA and SMA proper during action observation: Effects of instructions , 2005, NeuroImage.

[27]  P. Strick,et al.  Motor areas of the medial wall: a review of their location and functional activation. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[28]  R. Andersen,et al.  Functional analysis of human MT and related visual cortical areas using magnetic resonance imaging , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[29]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation: a neuromagnetic study. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[30]  Mikko Sams,et al.  Viewing speech modulates activity in the left SI mouth cortex , 2005, NeuroImage.

[31]  C. Frith,et al.  Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[32]  Jesper Andersson,et al.  Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic , 2005, NeuroImage.

[33]  Norihiro Sadato,et al.  Role of the superior temporal region in human visual motion perception. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[34]  Nava Rubin,et al.  Brain areas selective for both observed and executed movements. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  Emily S. Cross,et al.  Building a motor simulation de novo: Observation of dance by dancers , 2006, NeuroImage.

[36]  R. C. Oldfield THE PERCEPTION OF CAUSALITY , 1963 .

[37]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  The detection of contingency and animacy from simple animations in the human brain. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[38]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  D. Spalding The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.

[40]  Y. Paulignan,et al.  An Interference Effect of Observed Biological Movement on Action , 2003, Current Biology.

[41]  D I Perrett,et al.  Frameworks of analysis for the neural representation of animate objects and actions. , 1989, The Journal of experimental biology.

[42]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Specific Involvement of Human Parietal Systems and the Amygdala in the Perception of Biological Motion , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neural Circuits Involved in the Recognition of Actions Performed by Nonconspecifics: An fMRI Study , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  H. Leventhal,et al.  A perceptual-motor theory of emotion , 1984 .

[45]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  fMR-adaptation: a tool for studying the functional properties of human cortical neurons. , 2001, Acta psychologica.

[46]  C. Heyes Causes and consequences of imitation , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[47]  P. Strick,et al.  Imaging the premotor areas , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[48]  T. Allison,et al.  Brain Activity Evoked by the Perception of Human Walking: Controlling for Meaningful Coherent Motion , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  B. Mesquita,et al.  Adjustment to Chronic Diseases and Terminal Illness Health Psychology : Psychological Adjustment to Chronic Disease , 2006 .

[50]  R. Blake,et al.  Brain Areas Active during Visual Perception of Biological Motion , 2002, Neuron.

[51]  D. Perrett,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.8 No.11 November 2004 Demystifying social cognition: a Hebbian perspective , 2022 .

[52]  T. Allison,et al.  Functional anatomy of biological motion perception in posterior temporal cortex: an FMRI study of eye, mouth and hand movements. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[53]  Gian Luca Romani,et al.  Neural systems underlying observation of humanly impossible movements: an FMRI study. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[54]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[55]  R. Passingham,et al.  Action observation and acquired motor skills: an FMRI study with expert dancers. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[56]  Christian Keysers,et al.  The anthropomorphic brain: The mirror neuron system responds to human and robotic actions , 2007, NeuroImage.

[57]  F. J. McGuigan,et al.  Cognitive psychophysiology : principles of covert behavior , 1980 .

[58]  M. Sereno,et al.  Point-Light Biological Motion Perception Activates Human Premotor Cortex , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[59]  Helen E. Savaki,et al.  Observation of action: grasping with the mind's hand , 2004, NeuroImage.

[60]  Frank Rösler,et al.  The Principle of Code-Specific Memory Representations , 2003 .

[61]  J. Decety,et al.  Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: A meta‐analysis , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[62]  F. Heider,et al.  An experimental study of apparent behavior , 1944 .

[63]  J. Decety,et al.  Top down effect of strategy on the perception of human biological motion: a pet investigation. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[64]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Activity in fusiform face area modulated as a function of working memory load. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[65]  D Yves von Cramon,et al.  Sequences of Abstract Nonbiological Stimuli Share Ventral Premotor Cortex with Action Observation and Imagery , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[66]  Brice Isableu,et al.  Embodied spatial transformations: "body analogy" for the mental rotation of objects. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[67]  Peter Erhard,et al.  Transmodal Sensorimotor Networks during Action Observation in Professional Pianists , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[68]  G. Johansson Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .

[69]  A. Damasio Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: A systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition , 1989, Cognition.

[70]  C. Frith,et al.  Movement and Mind: A Functional Imaging Study of Perception and Interpretation of Complex Intentional Movement Patterns , 2000, NeuroImage.

[71]  B. Postle,et al.  Prefrontal cortical contributions to working memory: evidence from event-related fMRI studies , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[72]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Distinguishing subregions of the human MT+ complex using visual fields and pursuit eye movements. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[73]  R. Hari,et al.  Modulated Activation of the Human SI and SII Cortices during Observation of Hand Actions , 2002, NeuroImage.

[74]  T. Schenk,et al.  Visual motion perception after brain damage: II. Deficits in form-from-motion perception , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[75]  G. Orban,et al.  Specificity of regions processing biological motion , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[76]  D. V. Cramon,et al.  Functional–anatomical concepts of human premotor cortex: evidence from fMRI and PET studies , 2003, NeuroImage.

[77]  R. Schubotz Prediction of external events with our motor system: towards a new framework , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.