The influence of prior reputation and reciprocity on dynamic trust-building in adults with and without autism spectrum disorder

The present study was designed to investigate the effects of reputational priors and direct reciprocity on the dynamics of trust building in adults with (N = 17) and without (N = 25) autism spectrum disorder (ASD) using a multi-round Trust Game (MTG). On each round, participants, who played as investors, were required to maximize their benefits by updating their prior expectations (the partner's positive or negative reputation), based on the partner's directed reciprocity, and adjusting their own investment decisions accordingly. Results showed that reputational priors strongly oriented the initial decision to trust, operationalized as the amount of investment the investor shares with the counterpart. However, while typically developed participants were mainly affected by the direct reciprocity, and rapidly adopted the optimal Tit-for-Tat strategy, participants with ASD continued to rely on reputational priors throughout the game, even when experience of the counterpart's actual behavior contradicted their prior-based expectations. In participants with ASD, the effect of the reputational prior never disappeared, and affected judgments of trustworthiness and reciprocity of the partner even after completion of the game. Moreover, the weight of prior reputation positively correlated with the severity of the ASD participant's social impairments while the reciprocity score negatively correlated with the severity of repetitive and stereotyped behaviors, as measured by the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R). In line with Bayesian theoretical accounts, the present findings indicate that individuals with ASD have difficulties encoding incoming social information and using it to revise and flexibly update prior social expectations, and that this deficit might severely hinder social learning and everyday life interactions.

[1]  D. Burr,et al.  No rapid audiovisual recalibration in adults on the autism spectrum , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[2]  E. Koechlin,et al.  Neural coding of prior expectations in hierarchical intention inference , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[3]  J. Skewes,et al.  Perceptual inference and autistic traits , 2015, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[4]  J. Wagemans,et al.  Precise minds in uncertain worlds: predictive coding in autism. , 2014, Psychological review.

[5]  J. Piven,et al.  Abnormal Processing of Social Information from Faces in Autism , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  M. Nowak,et al.  Evolution of indirect reciprocity , 2005, Nature.

[7]  Angela Sirigu,et al.  Promoting social behavior with oxytocin in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[8]  Kang Lee,et al.  Children with autism spectrum disorder are more trusting than typically developing children. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[9]  Colin Camerer Behavioral Game Theory: Experiments in Strategic Interaction , 2003 .

[10]  Jonathan W. Peirce,et al.  PsychoPy—Psychophysics software in Python , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[11]  C. Gillberg,et al.  The Asperger Syndrome (and High-Functioning Autism) Diagnostic Interview (ASDI): A Preliminary Study of a New Structured Clinical Interview , 2001, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[12]  Danielle M Shore,et al.  Do social utility judgments influence attentional processing? , 2013, Cognition.

[13]  Jing Li,et al.  The relationship between moral judgment and cooperation in children with high-functioning autism , 2014, Scientific Reports.

[14]  M. Milinski,et al.  Cooperation through image scoring in humans. , 2000, Science.

[15]  M. Deutsch,et al.  Trust, trustworthiness, and the F scale. , 1960, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.

[16]  Jens Riegelsberger,et al.  The mechanics of trust: A framework for research and design , 2005, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[17]  M. Delgado,et al.  Perceptions of moral character modulate the neural systems of reward during the trust game , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  V. Braithwaite,et al.  The Long-Term Benefits of Human Generosity in Indirect Reciprocity , 2002, Current Biology.

[19]  M. Leboyer,et al.  Faux Pas Detection and Intentional Action in Asperger Syndrome. A Replication on a French Sample , 2009, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[20]  E. Hill,et al.  Executive processes in Asperger syndrome: Patterns of performance in a multiple case series , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  P. Avesani,et al.  Reputational Priors Magnify Striatal Responses to Violations of Trust , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  E. Koechlin,et al.  What Are They Up To? The Role of Sensory Evidence and Prior Knowledge in Action Understanding , 2011, PloS one.

[23]  Daniel M. Bartels,et al.  The Costs and Benefits of Calculation and Moral Rules , 2009, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[24]  M. Leboyer,et al.  Judgment of Intentionality and Moral Evaluation in Individuals with High Functioning Autism , 2011 .

[25]  U. Frith,et al.  Mindblind Eyes: An Absence of Spontaneous Theory of Mind in Asperger Syndrome , 2009, Science.

[26]  B. Gallhofer,et al.  Social Interaction Behavior in ADHD in Adults in a Virtual Trust Game , 2016, Journal of attention disorders.

[27]  C. Feinstein,et al.  Plasma oxytocin levels in autistic children , 1998, Biological Psychiatry.

[28]  W. Hamilton,et al.  The Evolution of Cooperation , 1984 .

[29]  Francesca Ervas,et al.  Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorders Do Not Use Social Stereotypes in Irony Comprehension , 2014, PloS one.

[30]  A. Todorov,et al.  The functional basis of face evaluation , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[31]  G. Rhodes,et al.  Appearance-based trust behaviour is reduced in children with autism spectrum disorder , 2015, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[32]  Xiaobing Zou,et al.  Distrust and retaliatory deception in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2014 .

[33]  J. Hohwy,et al.  Context sensitivity in action decreases along the autism spectrum: a predictive processing perspective , 2015, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[34]  M. Leboyer,et al.  The Role of Causal and Intentional Judgments in Moral Reasoning in Individuals with High Functioning Autism , 2013, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[35]  Mirella Dapretto,et al.  Intact recognition, but attenuated adaptation, for biological motion in youth with autism spectrum disorder , 2016, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[36]  M. Deutsch Trust and suspicion , 1958 .

[37]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Human Neuroscience Hypothesis and Theory Article an Aberrant Precision Account of Autism , 2022 .

[38]  Joyce E. Berg,et al.  Trust, Reciprocity, and Social History , 1995 .

[39]  S. Bourgeois-Gironde,et al.  Discriminating strategic reciprocity and acquired trust in the repeated trust-game , 2011 .

[40]  Flaminio Squazzoni,et al.  Reputational cues in repeated trust games , 2009 .

[41]  B. Leventhal,et al.  The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Generic: A Standard Measure of Social and Communication Deficits Associated with the Spectrum of Autism , 2000, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[42]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Learning the value of information in an uncertain world , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[43]  D. Burr,et al.  When the world becomes ‘too real’: a Bayesian explanation of autistic perception , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[44]  A. Couteur,et al.  Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders , 1994, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[45]  Claire L. Fox,et al.  Construction and validation of a children's interpersonal trust belief scale , 2005 .

[46]  S. Quartz,et al.  Getting to Know You: Reputation and Trust in a Two-Person Economic Exchange , 2005, Science.

[47]  M. Leboyer,et al.  Reduced sensitivity to social priors during action prediction in adults with autism spectrum disorders , 2017, Cognition.

[48]  S. Baron-Cohen Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind , 1997 .

[49]  M. Nowak,et al.  Cooperate without looking: Why we care what people think and not just what they do , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[50]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger Syndrome/High-Functioning Autism, Malesand Females, Scientists and Mathematicians , 2001, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[51]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ? , 1985, Cognition.

[52]  M. Stokes,et al.  Gender differences in emotionality and sociability in children with autism spectrum disorders , 2014, Molecular Autism.

[53]  Janet B W Williams Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders , 2013 .

[54]  M. Carter Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed. , 2014 .

[55]  M. Leboyer,et al.  Moral judgment in adults with autism spectrum disorders , 2011, Cognition.

[56]  John O. Willis,et al.  Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence , 2014 .