Cultural modulation of self-referential brain activity for personality traits and social identities

Cross-cultural studies have shown that personality traits are less central and social identities are more important to the selfhood of collectivistic people. However, most cultural neuroscience studies using the self-reference effect (SRE) paradigm have only used personality traits to explore cultural differences in the neural circuits of self-referential processes. In the present study, we used both personality traits and social identities as stimuli in the SRE paradigm and investigated whether and how one's cultural orientation (i.e., individualism vs. collectivism) affects the SRE in the brain. The results showed that the medial prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate, bilateral temporoparietal regions, and precuneus were involved in self-representation for both personality traits and social identities. Importantly, cultural orientation predicted differential activation patterns in these regions. Collectivists showed stronger activation in the left temporoparietal regions than individualists, who mainly recruited the medial prefrontal regions. Our findings suggest that the personal and social self share common neural substrates, the activation of which can be modulated by one's cultural orientation.

[1]  Kim Gregory Who is this , 2011 .

[2]  J. Decety,et al.  The Role of the Right Temporoparietal Junction in Social Interaction: How Low-Level Computational Processes Contribute to Meta-Cognition , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[3]  M. Iacoboni,et al.  The self and social cognition: the role of cortical midline structures and mirror neurons , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[4]  J. Decety,et al.  The power of simulation: Imagining one's own and other's behavior , 2006, Brain Research.

[5]  Jane F. Banfield,et al.  Medial prefrontal activity predicts memory for self. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[7]  H. Sackeim,et al.  Parietal cortex and representation of the mental Self. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[8]  Robert T. Schultz,et al.  Self, mother and abstract other: An fMRI study of reflective social processing , 2008, NeuroImage.

[9]  R. Nisbett,et al.  Culture and systems of thought: holistic versus analytic cognition. , 2001, Psychological review.

[10]  C. Frith,et al.  Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[11]  J. Tsai Ideal Affect: Cultural Causes and Behavioral Consequences , 2007, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[12]  C. Neil Macrae,et al.  The Link between Social Cognition and Self-referential Thought in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[14]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Cultural Influences on Neural Substrates of Attentional Control , 2008, Psychological science.

[15]  Georg Northoff,et al.  Self-referential processing in our brain—A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self , 2006, NeuroImage.

[16]  Ayse K. Uskul,et al.  Ecocultural basis of cognition: Farmers and fishermen are more holistic than herders , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[17]  M. Gelfand,et al.  Horizontal and Vertical Dimensions of Individualism and Collectivism: A Theoretical and Measurement Refinement , 1995 .

[18]  Andrew N. Meltzoff,et al.  Neural circuits involved in imitation and perspective-taking , 2006, NeuroImage.

[19]  Steve Majerus,et al.  Modulation of medial prefrontal and inferior parietal cortices when thinking about past, present, and future selves , 2010, Social neuroscience.

[20]  B. Keysar,et al.  The Effect of Culture on Perspective Taking , 2007, Psychological science.

[21]  B. Wagar,et al.  Culture, memory, and the self: An analysis of the personal and collective self in long-term memory , 2003 .

[22]  M. Brewer,et al.  Hierarchical Evolutionary Theory: There Is an Alternative, and It's Not Creationism , 1995 .

[23]  Bryan T. Denny,et al.  Medial prefrontal activity differentiates self from close others. , 2006, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[24]  Uta Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.

[25]  Elizabeth A. Hardie,et al.  The development and validation of the Relational, Individual, and Collective self‐aspects (RIC) Scale , 2000 .

[26]  S. Cousins Culture and self-perception in Japan and the United States. , 1989 .

[27]  Nathalie Boddaert,et al.  Autism, the superior temporal sulcus and social perception , 2006, Trends in Neurosciences.

[28]  Jin Fan,et al.  Neural basis of cultural influence on self-representation , 2007, NeuroImage.

[29]  T. B. Rogers,et al.  Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. , 1977, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[30]  H. Markus,et al.  Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. , 1991 .

[31]  J. Decety,et al.  How Would You Feel versus How Do You Think She Would Feel? A Neuroimaging Study of Perspective-Taking with Social Emotions , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  D. R. Lehman,et al.  Is there a universal need for positive self-regard? , 1999, Psychological review.

[33]  Claus Lamm,et al.  The Neural Substrate of Human Empathy: Effects of Perspective-taking and Cognitive Appraisal , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  Sterling C. Johnson,et al.  Metacognitive evaluation, self-relevance, and the right prefrontal cortex , 2004, NeuroImage.

[35]  C. N. Macrae,et al.  Finding the Self? An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[36]  G. Hofstede Motivation, leadership, and organization: Do American theories apply abroad? , 1980 .

[37]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  Dissociating medial frontal and posterior cingulate activity during self-reflection. , 2006, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[38]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  A PET Exploration of the Neural Mechanisms Involved in Reciprocal Imitation , 2002, NeuroImage.

[39]  D. Trafimow,et al.  Some tests of the distinction between the private self and the collective self. , 1991 .

[40]  G. Shulman,et al.  Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[41]  Jean-Luc Anton,et al.  Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox , 2010 .

[42]  Shihui Han,et al.  Self-Construal Priming Modulates Neural Substrates of Self-Awareness , 2007, Psychological science.

[43]  J. Decety,et al.  Effect of subjective perspective taking during simulation of action: a PET investigation of agency , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[44]  Heather M. Coon,et al.  Rethinking individualism and collectivism: evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. , 2002, Psychological bulletin.

[45]  Dana Samson,et al.  Left temporoparietal junction is necessary for representing someone else's belief , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[46]  Joan Y. Chiao,et al.  Neural basis of individualistic and collectivistic views of self , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[47]  Michael J. Martinez,et al.  Bias between MNI and Talairach coordinates analyzed using the ICBM‐152 brain template , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[48]  R. Nisbett,et al.  Running Head: CULTURE AND THOUGHT Culture and Systems of Thought: Holistic vs. Analytic Cognition , 2001 .

[49]  R. Turner,et al.  Characterizing Evoked Hemodynamics with fMRI , 1995, NeuroImage.

[50]  Jeff T. Larsen,et al.  Perceiving an Object and Its Context in Different Cultures , 2003, Psychological science.

[51]  H. Triandis The Self and Social Behavior in Differing Cultural Contexts , 1989 .

[52]  Richard E. Lucas,et al.  Personality, culture, and subjective well-being: emotional and cognitive evaluations of life. , 2003, Annual review of psychology.

[53]  M. Brewer,et al.  Who is this "We"? Levels of collective identity and self representations. , 1996 .

[54]  N. Anderson Likableness ratings of 555 personality-trait words. , 1968, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[55]  E. Kelly Individualism and collectivism , 1901 .

[56]  J. Townsend,et al.  Decision field theory: a dynamic-cognitive approach to decision making in an uncertain environment. , 1993, Psychological review.

[57]  D. Cohen,et al.  As Seen by the Other … : Perspectives on the Self in the Memories and Emotional Perceptions of Easterners and Westerners , 2002, Psychological science.

[58]  J. Sommerville,et al.  Shared representations between self and other: a social cognitive neuroscience view , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[59]  Zhang Li,et al.  Dynamic Cultural Influences on Neural Representations of the Self , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[60]  S. Heine,et al.  Self as cultural product: an examination of East Asian and North American selves. , 2001, Journal of personality.

[61]  Serena Chen,et al.  The relational self revealed: integrative conceptualization and implications for interpersonal life. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.