Looming detectors in the human visual pathway

Abstract Inspecting a target whose size oscillates about a constant mean value selectively depresses visual sensitivity to oscillating size. The effect transfers from positive to negative contrast and vice versa. This depression cannot be attributed to fatigued movement detectors. We propose that there are, in the human visual system, channels in which information as to changing size is selectively processed. This notion is consistent with the existence of two neural organizations (e.g., two classes of single neurons) selectively sensitive to increasing and to decreasing size, respectively.

[1]  P. O. Bishop,et al.  Responses to visual contours: spatio‐temporal aspects of excitation in the receptive fields of simple striate neurones , 1971, The Journal of physiology.

[2]  D. Regan,et al.  Selective adaptation in stereoscopic depth perception. , 1973, The Journal of physiology.

[3]  D Regan,et al.  The dissociation of sideways movements from movements in depth: psychophysics. , 1973, Vision research.

[4]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Size Adaptation: A New Aftereffect , 1969, Science.

[5]  C. Tyler,et al.  Frequency response characteristics for sinusoidal movement in the fovea and periphery , 1972 .

[6]  D Regan,et al.  Some dynamic features of depth perception. , 1973, Vision research.

[7]  Charles Wheatstone,et al.  I. The Bakerian Lecture.— Contributions to the physiology of vision.— Part the second. On some remarkable, and hitherto unobserved, phenomena of binocular vision (continued) , 1852, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London.

[8]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.

[9]  A. Pantle,et al.  Velocity-sensitive elements in human vision: initial psychophysical evidence. , 1968, Vision research.

[10]  A Pantle,et al.  Flicker adaptation. I. Effect on visual sensitivity to temporal fluctuations of light intensity. , 1971, Vision research.

[11]  M. Cynader,et al.  Neurones in cat parastriate cortex sensitive to the direction of motion in three‐dimensional space , 1978, The Journal of physiology.

[12]  Charles Wheatstone,et al.  Contributions to the Physiology of Vision. , 1837 .

[13]  R A Smith,et al.  Adaptation of visual contrast sensitivity to specific temporal frequencies. , 1970, Vision research.

[14]  D Regan,et al.  The relation between discrimination and sensitivity in the perception of motion in depth. , 1975, The Journal of physiology.

[15]  B. Julesz Binocular depth perception of computer-generated patterns , 1960 .

[16]  C. Wheatstone XVIII. Contributions to the physiology of vision. —Part the first. On some remarkable, and hitherto unobserved, phenomena of binocular vision , 1962, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London.

[17]  D. Regan,et al.  Evidence for the existence of neural mechanisms selectively sensitive to the direction of movement in space , 1973, The Journal of physiology.

[18]  D. Regan,et al.  Separable aftereffects of changing-size and motion-in-depth: Different neural mechanisms? , 1979, Vision Research.

[19]  A. Pantle,et al.  Physiological Basis of Motion Perception , 1978 .

[20]  D. Regan,et al.  Illusory motion in depth: Aftereffect of adaptation to changing size , 1978, Vision Research.

[21]  Whitman Richards,et al.  Broad band spatial filters in the human visual system , 1975, Vision Research.

[22]  S. Zeki Cells responding to changing image size and disparity in the cortex of the rhesus monkey , 1974, The Journal of physiology.

[23]  D. Regan,et al.  Disparity Detectors in Human Depth Perception: Evidence for Directional Selectivity , 1973, Science.