Early socio–emotional development: Contingency perception and the social-biofeedback model.

The past century of theory about human development has placed much responsibility for normal socio-emotional development on the social interactions experienced in infancy (e.g., Bandura, 1992; Bowlby, 1969; Bruner, 1990; Freud, 1949; Skinner, 1948; Stern, 1985; Trevarthen, 1979; Watson, 1930). The reliance on nurture over nature in each of these theories may need to be tempered in light of some recent proposals about a variety of richly structured innate mechanisms to interpret social stimulation [e.g., Leslie's (1987, 1994) theory of mind module; Baron-Cohen's (1995) detectors for perceiving another person's intention and eye direction; Meltzoff's (Meltzoff & Gopnik, 1993; Meltzoff & Moore, 1977, 1989) neonatal imitation mechanism; or Gergely and Csibra's (1996, 1997; Csibra & Gergely, in press) teleological stance for interpreting another's action]. Even if incorporating one or more of these specific interpretive mechanisms, however, these diverse theories will surely continue to rely heavily on an assumption they share, at least implicitly, to the effect that human infants are sensitive to the existence of contingencies between their behavior and environmental events. The capacity to accurately interpret stimulation as contingent or not could well be viewed as the most fundamental of an infant's arsenal of innate modules for interpreting early sensory stimulation. Whether as a means to establish a basis of conditioning (e.g., Bandura, Skinner) or a basis for building representations of social relations (e.g., Bowlby, Bruner, Trevarthen), an infant's capacity to detect contingency is taken for granted.

[1]  D. Spalding The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.

[2]  W. Bion,et al.  Learning from experience , 2007 .

[3]  L. Lunsky The Self and the Object World. , 1965 .

[4]  J. Bruner,et al.  Studies In Cognitive Growth , 1966 .

[5]  H. Birch,et al.  Visual differentiation, intersensory integration, and voluntary motor control. , 1967, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[6]  J. Watson Memory and "contingency analysis" in infant learning. , 1967 .

[7]  Wilfred R. Bion,et al.  A theory of thinking , 2021, The Complete Works of W. R. Bion.

[8]  Stephen Hughes Attachment and Loss. Vol. I , 1969, Mental Health.

[9]  J. Bowlby Attachment and loss: retrospect and prospect. , 1969, The American journal of orthopsychiatry.

[10]  C. K. Rovee,et al.  Conjugate reinforcement of infant exploratory behavior. , 1969, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[11]  N. Miller Learning of visceral and glandular responses. , 1969, Science.

[12]  L. Dicara Learning in the autonomic nervous system. , 1970, Scientific American.

[13]  H. Kohut,et al.  The analysis of the self , 1971 .

[14]  C. T. Ramey,et al.  Delayed reinforcement and vocalization rates of infants. , 1971, Child development.

[15]  W. S. Millar,et al.  A study of operant conditioning under delayed reinforcement in early infancy. , 1972, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[16]  G. W. Rever Attachment and Loss. Vol. 1. Attachment , 1972 .

[17]  J. Watson Smiling, cooing, and "the game." , 1972 .

[18]  M. Papoušek,et al.  Mirror image and self-recognition in young human infants: I. A method of experimental analysis. , 1974, Developmental psychobiology.

[19]  T. Brazelton,et al.  The origins of reciprocity : The early mother-infant interaction , 1974 .

[20]  M. S. Mahler,et al.  The Psychological Birth of the Human Infant: Symbiosis and Individuation , 1975 .

[21]  C. K. Rovee,et al.  Effects of quantitative shifts in a visual reinforcer on the instrumental response of infants. , 1976, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[22]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: I , 1977 .

[23]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .

[24]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Imitation of Facial and Manual Gestures by Human Neonates , 1977, Science.

[25]  T. Brazelton,et al.  Mutuality in mother-infant interaction. , 1977, The Journal of communication.

[26]  T. Brazelton,et al.  The infant's response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. , 1978, Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry.

[27]  C. E. Izard Emotions as motivations: an evolutionary-developmental perspective. , 1978, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.

[28]  A. Lock Action, gesture, and symbol : the emergence of language , 1978 .

[29]  Michael Lewis,et al.  Self-Knowledge and Emotional Development , 1978 .

[30]  N. Miller,et al.  Biofeedback and visceral learning. , 1978, Annual review of psychology.

[31]  T. Field Differential behavioral and cardiac responses of 3-month-old infants to a mirror and peer. , 1979 .

[32]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Intermodal matching by human neonates , 1979, Nature.

[33]  E. Bates,et al.  The emergence of intentional communication , 1979 .

[34]  K. Bloom Evaluation of infant vocal conditioning. , 1979, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[35]  J. Osofsky Handbook of infant development , 1979 .

[36]  C. Trevarthen Communication and cooperation in early infancy: a description of primary intersubjectivity , 1979 .

[37]  J. Watson,et al.  The effect of delayed feedback on infant learning reexamined. , 1979, Child development.

[38]  B. Gholson The cognitive-developmental basis of human learning : studies in hypothesis testing , 1980 .

[39]  Klaus Immelmann,et al.  Behavioral development : the Bielefeld interdisciplinary project , 1981 .

[40]  J. Gottman,et al.  The Analysis of Dominance and Bidirectionality in Social Development. , 1981 .

[41]  M. Bornstein,et al.  Development in Infancy , 1982 .

[42]  P. Ekman Emotion in the human face , 1982 .

[43]  M. Mahler,et al.  Thoughts on the Emergence of the Sense of Self, with Particular Emphasis on the Body Self , 1982, Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association.

[44]  M. Lewis Origins of Intelligence , 1983 .

[45]  A. Decasper,et al.  Human newborns' perception of male voices: preference, discrimination, and reinforcing value. , 1984, Developmental psychobiology.

[46]  N. Fox,et al.  Social perception in infants , 1985 .

[47]  D. Stern The interpersonal world of infant , 1985 .

[48]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ? , 1985, Cognition.

[49]  Infants' responses to mothers' imitative behaviors , 1985 .

[50]  V. Demos Crying in early infancy: An illustration of the motivational function of affect. , 1986 .

[51]  T. Brazelton,et al.  Affective development in infancy. , 1986 .

[52]  R. Matias,et al.  Face-to-face interactions of depressed mothers and their infants. , 1986, New directions for child development.

[53]  Tiffany M Field,et al.  Maternal Depression and Infant Disturbance , 1986 .

[54]  A. Leslie Pretense and representation: The origins of "theory of mind." , 1987 .

[55]  H. Papoušek,et al.  Intuitive parenting: A dialectic counterpart to the infant's integrative competence. , 1987 .

[56]  C. Izard,et al.  Perspectives on emotional development I: Differential emotions theory of early emotional development. , 1987 .

[57]  C. Rovee-Collier Learning and memory in infancy. , 1987 .

[58]  B. Bettes,et al.  Maternal depression and motherese: temporal and intonational features. , 1988, Child development.

[59]  B. Beebe,et al.  The contribution of mother-infant mutual influence to the origins of self- and object representations. , 1988 .

[60]  T. Field,et al.  Infants of depressed mothers show "depressed" behavior even with nondepressed adults. , 1988, Child development.

[61]  J. Cohn,et al.  Mother infant face to face interaction influence is bidirectional and unrelated to periodic cycles in either partner's behavior , 1988 .

[62]  Marie Vasek,et al.  Contextual Influences on Imitative Interactions between Mothers and Infants , 1989 .

[63]  Jeffery F. Cohn,et al.  Infant-mother face-to-face interaction: age and gender differences in coordination and the occurrence of miscoordination , 1989 .

[64]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Imitation in Newborn Infants: Exploring the Range of Gestures Imitated and the Underlying Mechanisms. , 1989, Developmental psychology.

[65]  E. Tronick Emotions and emotional communication in infants. , 1989, The American psychologist.

[66]  C. Malatesta,et al.  The development of emotion expression during the first two years of life. , 1989, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[67]  H. Papoušek,et al.  Forms and functions of vocal matching in interactions between mothers and their precanonical infants , 1989 .

[68]  U. Frith Autism: Explaining the Enigma , 2005 .

[69]  Discrimination learning during the first year: stimulus and positional cues. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[70]  M. Main,et al.  Parents' unresolved traumatic experiences are related to infant disorganized attachment status: Is frightened and/or frightening parental behavior the linking mechanism? , 1990 .

[71]  A. Meltzoff Foundations for developing a concept of self: The role of imitation in relating self to other and the value of social mirroring, social modeling, and self practice in infancy. , 1990 .

[72]  D. Cicchetti,et al.  Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research, and intervention. , 1990 .

[73]  J. Bruner Acts of meaning , 1990 .

[74]  Robert M. Thorndike,et al.  Origins of Intelligence and its Measurement , 1990 .

[75]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  The perception of facial structure in infancy. , 1991 .

[76]  B. Pennington,et al.  Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind. , 1991, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[77]  Paul Ekman,et al.  Facial Expressions of Emotion: New Findings, New Questions , 1992 .

[78]  S. Feinman Social Referencing and the Social Construction of Reality in Infancy , 1992 .

[79]  A. Bandura Social cognitive theory of social referencing. , 1992 .

[80]  L Murray,et al.  The impact of postnatal depression on infant development. , 1992, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[81]  J. Jaffe,et al.  A dyadic systems view of communication. , 1992 .

[82]  M. Peláez-Nogueras,et al.  Social Referencing as a Learned Process , 1992 .

[83]  T. Bower,et al.  Newborns Form “Prototypes” in Less Than 1 Minute , 1993 .

[84]  J. Pickens,et al.  Facial Expressivity in Infants of Depressed Mothers. , 1993 .

[85]  Robert W. Mitchell,et al.  Self-Awareness in Animals and Humans: Developmental Perspectives , 1994 .

[86]  A. Leslie Mapping the mind: ToMM, ToBY, and Agency: Core architecture and domain specificity , 1994 .

[87]  L. Schauble,et al.  Beyond Modularity: A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science. , 1994 .

[88]  T. Bower,et al.  Learning and Intermodal Transfer of Information in Newborns , 1994 .

[89]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Understanding other minds : perspectives from autism , 1994 .

[90]  S. Gelman,et al.  Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity In Cognition And Culture , 1994 .

[91]  T. Field,et al.  The effects of mother's physical and emotional unavailability on emotion regulation. , 1994, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[92]  John S. Watson Self-awareness in animals and humans: Detection of self: The perfect algorithm , 1994 .

[93]  C. Moore,et al.  Social Understanding at the End of the First Year of Life , 1994 .

[94]  R. Hobson Autism and the Development of Mind , 1995 .

[95]  Rachel Morgan,et al.  Spatial determinants in the perception of self-produced leg movements in 3- to 5-month-old infants. , 1995 .

[96]  Z. Nadasdy,et al.  Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age , 1995, Cognition.

[97]  Mark A. Schmuckler,et al.  Visual-Proprioceptive intermodal perception in infancy , 1996 .

[98]  P. Cooper,et al.  The impact of postnatal depression and associated adversity on early mother-infant interactions and later infant outcome. , 1996, Child development.

[99]  D. Muir,et al.  Infant sensitivity to adult eye direction. , 1996, Child development.

[100]  P. Fonagy,et al.  Playing with reality: I. Theory of mind and the normal development of psychic reality. , 1996, The International journal of psycho-analysis.

[101]  C. Moore,et al.  Intentional relations and social understanding , 1996, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[102]  Understanding rational actions in infancy: Teleological interpretations without mental attribution , 1996 .

[103]  J. Watson,et al.  The social biofeedback theory of parental affect-mirroring: the development of emotional self-awareness and self-control in infancy. , 1996, The International journal of psycho-analysis.

[104]  György Gergely,et al.  Teleological reasoning in infancy: The infant's naive theory of rational action A reply to Premack and Premack , 1997, Cognition.

[105]  J S Watson,et al.  Contingency and Its Two Indices Within Conditional Probability Analysis , 1997, The Behavior analyst.

[106]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Words, thoughts, and theories , 1997 .

[107]  D. Perrett,et al.  Eye-direction detection: A dissociation between geometric and joint attention skills in autism , 1997 .

[108]  L. Huffman Emotional Development: The Organization of Emotional Life in the Early Years , 1997 .

[109]  J. Jaffe,et al.  Mother—infant interaction structures and presymbolic self‐ and object representations , 1997 .

[110]  P Fonagy,et al.  Attachment and reflective function: Their role in self-organization , 1997, Development and Psychopathology.

[111]  S. Baron-Cohen Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind , 1997 .

[112]  P. Rochat,et al.  Are young infants sensitive to interpersonal contingency , 1998 .

[113]  M. Forrester Agency: Its role in mental development , 1999 .

[114]  G. Gergely,et al.  The obscure object of desire: 'nearly, but clearly not, like me': contingency preference in normal children versus children with autism. , 2001, Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic.