Infant's brain responses to live and televised action

Whether human infants perceive televised stimuli in the same way to live stimuli largely remains unknown. Action observation, which has been extensively confirmed to elicit activation of internal motor representation, provides a promising framework for investigating this issue. This 'mirror-matching' property has been found in the monkey premotor cortex as well as the premotor and primary motor cortices in human adults. Although larger activation in observing a live action compared to a televised action in adult subjects has been reported, it is unknown whether the same neural response is obtained from human infants. To address this issue, we first measured the activity of motor areas in adult subjects while viewing either a live or televised action of other people by using near-infrared spectroscopy. The motor areas that were activated when the subject themselves performed an action were also activated during action observation in the live setting, while this was not evident in the TV setting. We then conducted qualitatively the same experiment with 6- to 7-month-old infants. The infant's motor areas were significantly activated when observing a live person performing an action. Although we also found activation in the same area during action observation in the TV setting, the difference in activity between action observation and object-motion observation was significant only in the live setting. Our results are the first to demonstrate activation in motor areas during action observation in human infants. We suggest that human brain responds differently to the real world and the virtual world.

[1]  R. Passingham,et al.  Action observation and acquired motor skills: an FMRI study with expert dancers. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[2]  David A. Boas,et al.  A Quantitative Comparison of Simultaneous BOLD fMRI and NIRS Recordings during Functional Brain Activation , 2002, NeuroImage.

[3]  D. Yves von Cramon,et al.  Prefrontal activation due to Stroop interference increases during development—an event-related fNIRS study , 2004, NeuroImage.

[4]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Object representation in the ventral premotor cortex (area F5) of the monkey. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[6]  W. Prinz,et al.  The imitative mind : development, evolution, and brain bases , 2002 .

[7]  J. Piaget Play, dreams and imitation in childhood , 1951 .

[8]  Hellmuth Obrig,et al.  Separability and cross talk: optimizing dual wavelength combinations for near-infrared spectroscopy of the adult head , 2004, NeuroImage.

[9]  Helen E. Savaki,et al.  Observation of action: grasping with the mind's hand , 2004, NeuroImage.

[10]  Harlene Hayne,et al.  Deferred imitation by 6- and 9-month-old infants: more evidence for declarative memory. , 1999 .

[11]  Riitta Hari,et al.  Activation of the human primary motor cortex during observation of tool use , 2004, NeuroImage.

[12]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation: a neuromagnetic study. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[13]  R. Hari,et al.  Stronger reactivity of the human primary motor cortex during observation of live rather than video motor acts , 2001, Neuroreport.

[14]  J. Decety,et al.  Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a neuroimaging study , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[16]  Tony Charman,et al.  Gradations of emulation learning in infants' imitation of actions on objects. , 2005, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[17]  G. Taga,et al.  Brain imaging in awake infants by near-infrared optical topography , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[18]  David A. Boas,et al.  Frontal Lobe Activation during Object Permanence: Data from Near-Infrared Spectroscopy , 2002, NeuroImage.

[19]  T. Chartrand,et al.  Beyond the Perception-Behavior Link: The Ubiquitous Utility and Motivational Moderators of Nonconscious Mimicry. , 2005 .

[20]  Masako Okamoto,et al.  Three-dimensional probabilistic anatomical cranio-cerebral correlation via the international 10–20 system oriented for transcranial functional brain mapping , 2004, NeuroImage.

[21]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Imitation of Facial and Manual Gestures by Human Neonates , 1977, Science.

[22]  R. Hari,et al.  Modulated Activation of the Human SI and SII Cortices during Observation of Hand Actions , 2002, NeuroImage.

[23]  Ran R. Hassin,et al.  The New Unconscious , 2006 .

[24]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. , 1999, Science.

[25]  R. Gur,et al.  Age-related volumetric changes of brain gray and white matter in healthy infants and children. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[26]  Kazuo Hiraki,et al.  The parietal role in the sense of self-ownership with temporal discrepancy between visual and proprioceptive feedbacks , 2005, NeuroImage.

[27]  L. Craighero,et al.  Electrophysiology of Action Representation , 2004, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[28]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  Patricia Cohen,et al.  Television Viewing and Aggressive Behavior During Adolescence and Adulthood , 2002, Science.

[30]  A. Bandura,et al.  Imitation of film-mediated agressive models. , 1963, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.

[31]  J. Mehler,et al.  Sounds and silence: An optical topography study of language recognition at birth , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[32]  Kazuo Hiraki,et al.  Sustained decrease in oxygenated hemoglobin during video games in the dorsal prefrontal cortex: A NIRS study of children , 2006, NeuroImage.

[33]  Frithjof Kruggel,et al.  Age dependency of the hemodynamic response as measured by functional near-infrared spectroscopy , 2003, NeuroImage.

[34]  K. Kubota,et al.  Cortical Mapping of Gait in Humans: A Near-Infrared Spectroscopic Topography Study , 2001, NeuroImage.

[35]  J. Stevenson The cultural origins of human cognition , 2001 .

[36]  M. Tamura,et al.  Interpretation of near-infrared spectroscopy signals: a study with a newly developed perfused rat brain model. , 2001, Journal of applied physiology.

[37]  H. Hayne,et al.  Developmental changes in imitation from television during infancy. , 1999, Child development.

[38]  C. Rovee-Collier,et al.  Generalization of deferred imitation during the first year of life. , 2004, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[39]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Motor facilitation while observing hand actions: specificity of the effect and role of observer's orientation. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  Hellmuth Obrig,et al.  Towards a standard analysis for functional near-infrared imaging , 2004, NeuroImage.

[41]  V. Gallese The manifold nature of interpersonal relations: the quest for a common mechanism. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[42]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Premotor Cortex Activation during Observation and Naming of Familiar Tools , 1997, NeuroImage.

[43]  R. Hari,et al.  Temporal dynamics of cortical representation for action. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[44]  A. Meltzoff Imitation of televised models by infants. , 1988, Child development.

[45]  David A. Boas,et al.  Factors affecting the accuracy of near-infrared spectroscopy concentration calculations for focal changes in oxygenation parameters , 2003, NeuroImage.