Becoming a Social Partner With Peers: Cooperation and Social Understanding in One- and Two-Year-Olds: Toddler Peer Cooperation
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Peter Gärdenfors,et al. Co-operation and communication in apes and humans , 2003 .
[2] H. Wellman,et al. The acquisition of mental verbs: A systematic investigation of the first reference to mental state , 1983, Cognition.
[3] Maria Maudry,et al. Social relations between children of the same age during the first two years of life. , 1939 .
[4] M. Tomasello,et al. Fourteen-through 18-month-old infants di eren-tially imitate intentional and accidental actions , 1998 .
[5] Rebecca J. Brand,et al. Breaking the language barrier: an emergentist coalition model for the origins of word learning. , 2000, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.
[6] Constructing an understanding of mind with peers , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[7] M. Tomasello,et al. Unwilling versus unable: infants' understanding of intentional action. , 2005, Developmental psychology.
[8] K. Bard,et al. Maternal gestures with 20-month-old infants in two contexts. , 2005, Developmental science.
[9] M. Tomasello,et al. Understanding attention: 12- and 18-month-olds know what is new for other persons. , 2003, Developmental psychology.
[10] L. Youngblade,et al. Young children's understanding of other people's feelings and beliefs: individual differences and their antecedents. , 1991, Child development.
[11] D. Witherington,et al. Mothers' behavior modifications during pretense and their possible signal value for toddlers. , 2004, Developmental psychology.
[12] C. Howes. Peer interaction of young children , 1989 .
[13] H. Ross. Toddler peer relations: Differentiation of games and conflicts. , 1982 .
[14] B. Repacholi. Infants' use of attentional cues to identify the referent of another person's emotional expression. , 1998, Developmental psychology.
[15] Anne D. Pick,et al. Verbal encouragement and joint attention in 18-month-old infants , 2003 .
[16] D. Poulin-Dubois,et al. Toddlers' attention to intentions-in-action in learning novel action words. , 2002, Developmental psychology.
[17] G. Kochanska,et al. Development of mutual responsiveness between parents and their young children. , 2004, Child development.
[18] C. Moore,et al. Intentional relations and social understanding , 1996, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[19] M. Callanan,et al. Multiple labels for objects in conversations with young children: parents' language and children's developing expectations about word meanings. , 2004, Developmental psychology.
[20] Michael Tomasello,et al. Cooperative Problem‐Solving and Teaching in Preschoolers , 2001 .
[21] M. Tomasello,et al. Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. , 1998, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.
[22] D. Rutter,et al. Turn-taking in mother–infant interaction: An examination of vocalizations and gaze. , 1987 .
[23] M. Tomasello,et al. Understanding "prior intentions" enables two-year-olds to imitatively learn a complex task. , 2002, Child development.
[24] H. Bekkering,et al. Developmental psychology: Rational imitation in preverbal infants , 2002, Nature.
[25] Z. Nadasdy,et al. Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age , 1995, Cognition.
[26] L. Mo,et al. Relative roles of general and complementation language in theory-of-mind development: evidence from Cantonese and English. , 2004, Child development.
[27] B. Sodian,et al. Infants' Understanding of Looking, Pointing, and Reaching as Cues to Goal-Directed Action , 2004 .
[28] P. Harris,et al. Language and Children's Understanding of Mental States , 2005 .
[29] C. Eckerman,et al. Toddlers' emerging ways of achieving social coordinations with a peer. , 1989, Child development.
[30] M. Tomasello,et al. Twelve- and 18-month-olds copy actions in terms of goals. , 2005, Developmental science.
[31] A. Campbell,et al. A longitudinal study of gender-related cognition and behaviour. , 2004, Developmental science.
[32] Celia A. Brownell,et al. Peers and Play in Infants and Toddlers , 1992 .
[33] J. Asendorpf,et al. Self-Awareness and Other-Awareness: Mirror Self-Recognition and Synchronic Imitation among Unfamiliar Peers. , 1993 .
[34] Dare A. Baldwin,et al. Early referential understanding: Infants' ability to recognize referential acts for what they are. , 1993 .
[35] J. Astington,et al. A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. , 1999, Developmental psychology.
[36] M. Beeghly,et al. Talking About Internal States: The Acquisition of an Explicit Theory of Mind , 1982 .
[37] Edward Mueller,et al. The Origins of Social Skills and Interaction among Playgroup Toddlers. , 1977 .
[38] Amanda L. Woodward,et al. Infants’ developing understanding of the link between looker and object , 2003 .
[39] J. Carpendale,et al. Constructing an understanding of mind: The development of children's social understanding within social interaction , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[40] M. Tomasello,et al. Re-enacting intended acts: Comparing 12- and 18-month-olds , 1999 .
[41] H. Ross,et al. Communication within infant social games. , 1987 .
[42] Douglas K. Symons,et al. Mental state discourse, theory of mind, and the internalization of self–other understanding , 2004 .
[43] Patricia A. Smiley. Intention Understanding and Partner-Sensitive Behaviors in Young Children’s Peer Interactions , 2001 .
[44] A. Meltzoff. Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.
[45] E. Markman,et al. Young children's appreciation of the mental impact of their communicative signals. , 1997, Developmental psychology.
[46] P. Mundy. Motivation, self-regulation, and the neurodevelopment of intention sharing , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[47] M. Tomasello,et al. Twelve-month-olds point to share attention and interest. , 2004, Developmental science.
[48] Roger Bakeman,et al. Infants' conventionalized acts: Gestures and words with mothers and peers☆ , 1986 .
[49] M. Lewis,et al. Development of self-recognition, personal pronoun use, and pretend play during the 2nd year. , 2004, Child development.
[50] Celia A. Brownell,et al. Changes in cooperation and self-other differentiation during the second year. , 1990, Child development.
[51] M. Tomasello,et al. The role of language in the development of false belief understanding: a training study. , 2003, Child development.
[52] A. J. Caron,et al. Comprehension of the Referential Intent of Looking and Pointing Between 12 and 15 Months , 2002 .
[53] M G Pêcheux,et al. Maternal responsiveness to infants in three societies: the United States, France, and Japan. , 1992, Child development.
[54] L. Camaioni,et al. Content and structure in toddlers' social competence with peers from 12 to 36 months of age , 1991 .
[55] P. Mundy,et al. Assessing interactional competencies: The early social‐communication scales , 1982 .
[56] E. Visalberghi,et al. Solving a cooperation task without taking into account the partner's behavior: the case of capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). , 2000, Journal of comparative psychology.
[57] A. Woodward. Infants' ability to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors , 1999 .
[58] J. Jenkins,et al. A longitudinal investigation of the dynamics of mental state talk in families. , 2003, Child development.
[59] Michael Tomasello,et al. 12- and 18-month-old infants follow gaze to spaces behind barriers. , 2004, Developmental science.
[60] M. Tomasello,et al. Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[61] C. Brownell,et al. Early Peer Interaction: A Research Agenda , 1999 .
[62] E. Visalberghi,et al. Capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella fail to understand a cooperative task , 1997, Animal Behaviour.
[63] C. Eckerman,et al. Nonverbal imitation and toddlers' mastery of verbal means of achieving coordinated action , 1996 .
[64] P. Rochat,et al. Imitative games by 9-, 14-, and 18-month-old infants. , 2004 .
[65] Dale F. Hay,et al. Cooperative interactions and sharing between very young children and their parents. , 1979 .
[66] Celia A. Brownell,et al. Convergent developments: Cognitive-developmental correlates of growth in infant/toddler peer skills. , 1986 .
[67] Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al. An Emergentist Coalition Model for Word Learning , 2000 .
[68] M. J. Farrar,et al. Early language development and the emergence of a theory of mind∗ , 2002 .
[69] H. Wellman,et al. Infants' ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action , 2002, Cognition.
[70] K. Wynn,et al. Attribution of Dispositional States by 12-Month-Olds , 2003, Psychological science.
[71] L. Rocissano,et al. Dyadic synchrony and toddler compliance. , 1987 .
[72] H. Wellman,et al. Infants' understanding of occlusion of others' line-of-sight: Implications for an emerging theory of mind , 2004 .
[73] C. Eckerman,et al. How Toddler Peers Generate Coordinated Action: A Cross-Cultural Exploration , 1999 .
[74] L. Adamson,et al. Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interaction. , 1984, Child development.
[75] C. Boesch,et al. Hunting behavior of wild chimpanzees in the Taï National Park. , 1989, American journal of physical anthropology.
[76] M. Ricard,et al. Personal pronouns and perspective taking in toddlers , 1999, Journal of Child Language.
[77] A. Gopnik,et al. Early reasoning about desires: evidence from 14- and 18-month-olds. , 1997, Developmental psychology.