What Does It Take for an Infant to Learn How to Use a Tool by Observation?
暂无分享,去创建一个
Rana Esseily | Jacqueline Fagard | Lauriane Rat-Fischer | Eszter Somogyi | J. O'Regan | J. Fagard | E. Somogyi | R. Esseily | J. K. O’Regan | L. Rat-Fischer
[1] M. Tomasello,et al. Twelve- and 18-month-olds copy actions in terms of goals. , 2005, Developmental science.
[2] J. Lockman. A perception--action perspective on tool use development. , 2000, Child development.
[3] A. Meltzoff. Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.
[4] Harold Bekkering,et al. Imitation in infancy: rational or motor resonance? , 2011, Child development.
[5] E. Markman,et al. Precision of imitation as a function of preschoolers' understanding of the goal of the demonstration. , 2006, Developmental psychology.
[6] Amy Needham,et al. Learning about tools in infancy. , 2007, Developmental psychology.
[7] Jackie Chappell,et al. Making tools isn’t child’s play , 2011, Cognition.
[8] R. Keen. The development of problem solving in young children: a critical cognitive skill. , 2011, Annual review of psychology.
[9] M. Tomasello,et al. Fourteen-through 18-month-old infants di eren-tially imitate intentional and accidental actions , 1998 .
[10] Gisa Aschersleben,et al. The Development of Rational Imitation in 9- and 12-Month-Old Infants. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[11] R. Siegler,et al. I. The Geat Dvide , 2000 .
[12] Object retrieval through observational learning in 8-to 18-month-old infants , 2010 .
[13] J. Fagard,et al. Humour production may enhance observational learning of a new tool-use action in 18-month-old infants , 2016, Cognition & emotion.
[14] G. Aschersleben,et al. Imitating step by step: a detailed analysis of 9- to 15-month-olds' reproduction of a three-step action sequence. , 2007, Infant behavior & development.
[15] D. Levine,et al. A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. , 1999, Psychological review.
[16] A. Smitsman,et al. Affordances, perceptual complexity, and the development of tool use. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[17] G. Csibra,et al. Teleological reasoning in infancy: the naı̈ve theory of rational action , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[18] Andrew Whiten,et al. Emulation and "overemulation" in the social learning of causally opaque versus causally transparent tool use by 23- and 30-month-olds. , 2009, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[19] Andrew Whiten,et al. The transmission and evolution of experimental microcultures in groups of young children. , 2010, Developmental psychology.
[20] S. Bennett,et al. Developmental effects influencing observational modelling: A meta-analysis , 2007, Journal of sports sciences.
[21] Object retrieval through observational learning in 8- to 18-month-old infants. , 2010, Infant behavior & development.
[22] Jacqueline Fagard,et al. The emergence of tool use during the second year of life. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[23] Jacqueline Nadel,et al. Imitation in infancy , 2011 .
[24] J. Call,et al. Great apes select tools on the basis of their rigidity. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[25] Ann L. Brown,et al. Domain-Specific Principles Affect Learning and Transfer in Children , 1990, Cogn. Sci..
[26] The Imitation of Gestures , 1966 .
[27] J. Fagard,et al. Change in imitation for object manipulation between 10 and 12 months of age. , 2009, Developmental psychobiology.
[28] Gisa Aschersleben,et al. Do I get what you get? Learning about the effects of self-performed and observed actions in infancy , 2003, Consciousness and Cognition.
[29] Andrew Whiten,et al. Imitation of causally opaque versus causally transparent tool use by 3- and 5-year-old children , 2007 .
[30] Michael Tomasello,et al. Rational tool use and tool choice in human infants and great apes. , 2008, Child development.
[31] R. Siegler,et al. ACROSS THE GREAT DIVIDE : BRIDGING THE GAP BETWEEN UNDERSTANDING OF TODDLERS ' AND OLDER CHILDREN ' S THINKING Zhe , 2002 .
[32] J. Kevin O'Regan,et al. The emergence of use of a rake-like tool: a longitudinal study in human infants , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[33] K. Connolly,et al. The Emergence of a Tool-Using Skill in Infancy. , 1989 .
[34] The influence of peers on the tendency of 3- and 4-year-old children to over-imitate. , 2015, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[35] A. Meltzoff. Infant Imitation After a 1-Week Delay: Long-Term Memory for Novel Acts and Multiple Stimuli. , 1988, Developmental psychology.
[36] Lydia M. Hopper,et al. Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[37] B. Elsner. Infants' imitation of goal-directed actions: the role of movements and action effects. , 2007, Acta psychologica.
[38] Joëlle Provasi,et al. Do 9- and 12-month-olds learn means-ends relation by observing? , 2001 .
[39] M. Tomasello,et al. Processes of social learning in the tool use of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). , 1993, Journal of comparative psychology.
[40] P. Willatts. Development of means-end behavior in young infants: pulling a support to retrieve a distant object. , 1999, Developmental psychology.
[41] M. Tomasello,et al. Understanding "prior intentions" enables two-year-olds to imitatively learn a complex task. , 2002, Child development.
[42] G. Csibra,et al. Sensitivity to communicative relevance tells young children what to imitate. , 2009, Developmental science.
[43] H. Bekkering,et al. Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[44] Stephen C. Want,et al. Learning from other people's mistakes: causal understanding in learning to use a tool. , 2001, Child development.
[45] P. Willatts,et al. Development of means-end behavior in young infants: pulling a support to retrieve a distant object. , 1999, Developmental psychology.
[46] Understanding the experimenter's intention improves 16-month-olds’ observational learning of the use of a novel tool , 2013 .
[47] Lydia M. Hopper,et al. Observational learning of tool use in children: Investigating cultural spread through diffusion chains and learning mechanisms through ghost displays. , 2010, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[48] H. Bekkering,et al. Developmental psychology: Rational imitation in preverbal infants , 2002, Nature.
[49] A. RalfF.. Perseveration in Tool Use: A Window for Understanding the Dynamics of the Action-Selection Process , 2017 .
[50] C. Heyes,et al. Infants' behavioral reenactment of "failed attempts": exploring the roles of emulation learning, stimulus enhancement, and understanding of intentions. , 2002, Developmental psychology.
[51] Elizabeth Bates,et al. Perceptual aspects of tool using in infancy , 1980 .
[52] J. O'Regan,et al. The roles of observation and manipulation in learning to use a tool , 2015 .
[53] M. Tomasello,et al. Re-enacting intended acts: Comparing 12- and 18-month-olds , 1999 .
[54] When do infants understand that they can obtain a desired part of a composite object by grasping another part? , 2015, Infant behavior & development.
[55] Ad W. Smitsman,et al. Perseveration in Tool Use: A Window for Understanding the Dynamics of the Action-Selection Process , 2008 .
[56] E Abravanel,et al. Action imitation: the early phase of infancy. , 1976, Child development.
[57] C. Poulson,et al. Imitation in infancy: a critical review. , 1989, Advances in child development and behavior.
[58] E. Somogyi,et al. Mimicry Enhances Observational Learning in 16-Month-Old Infants , 2014, PloS one.
[59] S. Bennett,et al. Goal-directed imitation: the means to an end. , 2008, Acta psychologica.