Do 5-month-old infants see humans as material objects?

Infants expect objects to be solid and cohesive, and to move on continuous paths through space. In this study, we examine whether infants understand that human beings are material objects, subject to these same principles. We report that 5-month-old infants apply the constraint of continuous motion to inanimate blocks, but not to people. This suggests that young infants have two separate modes of construal: one for inanimate objects and another for humans.

[1]  A. Bigelow,et al.  Sensitivity to Social Contingency From Mothers and Strangers in 2‐, 4‐, and 6‐Month‐Old Infants , 2003 .

[2]  G. Csibra,et al.  Goal attribution without agency cues: the perception of ‘pure reason’ in infancy , 1999, Cognition.

[3]  E. Spelke,et al.  Origins of knowledge. , 1992, Psychological review.

[4]  R. Baillargeon,et al.  2.5-Month-Old Infants' Reasoning about When Objects Should and Should Not Be Occluded , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.

[5]  L. Kaufman,et al.  Distinguishing Between Animates And Inanimates: Not By Motion Alone , 1995 .

[6]  Lisa M. Oakes,et al.  Early Category and Concept Development: Making Sense of the Blooming, Buzzing Confusion , 2008 .

[7]  A. Premack,et al.  Causal cognition : a multidisciplinary debate , 1996 .

[8]  E. Spelke,et al.  Infants' knowledge of object motion and human action. , 1995 .

[9]  A. Meltzoff Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.

[10]  Marc D. Hauser,et al.  A nonhuman primate's expectations about object motion and destination: The importance of self-propelled movement and animacy , 1998 .

[11]  John E. Opfer,et al.  Identifying living and sentient kinds from dynamic information: the case of goal-directed versus aimless autonomous movement in conceptual change , 2002, Cognition.

[12]  D. Muir,et al.  A demonstration of gaze following in 3- to 6-month-olds , 1997 .

[13]  E. Spelke,et al.  Perceiving and reasoning about objects: Insights from infants , 1993 .

[14]  A. Leslie The Perception of Causality in Infants , 1982, Perception.

[15]  P. Bloom Descartes' Baby: How the Science of Child Development Explains What Makes Us Human , 2004 .

[16]  L. Cohen,et al.  How infants process addition and subtraction events , 2002 .

[17]  Rosaleen A. McCarthy,et al.  Spatial Representation: Problems in Philosophy and Psychology , 1993 .

[18]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Fourteen-through 18-month-old infants di eren-tially imitate intentional and accidental actions , 1998 .

[19]  A. Woodward Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach , 1998, Cognition.

[20]  Diane Poulin-Dubois,et al.  Infants' concept of animacy. , 1996 .

[21]  L. Oakes Development of Infants' Use of Continuity Cues in Their Perception of Causality. , 1994 .

[22]  H. Wellman,et al.  Infants' ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action , 2002, Cognition.

[23]  Jodie A. Baird,et al.  Infants parse dynamic action. , 2001, Child development.

[24]  E. Spelke,et al.  Object permanence in five-month-old infants , 1985, Cognition.

[25]  E. Spelke,et al.  Spatiotemporal continuity, smoothness of motion and object identity in infancy , 1995 .

[26]  B. Sodian,et al.  Do infants apply the principle of rational action to human agents , 2004 .

[27]  D. Poulin-Dubois,et al.  Of bears and men: Infants’ knowledge of conventional and metaphorical gender stereotypes , 2002 .

[28]  S. Carey,et al.  Whose gaze will infants follow? Features that elicit gaze-following in twelve-month-olds , 1996 .

[29]  Renate Zangl,et al.  The Human First Hypothesis: Identification of Conspecifics and Individuation of Objects in the Young Infant , 2002, Cognitive Psychology.

[30]  K. Wynn,et al.  Attribution of Dispositional States by 12-Month-Olds , 2003, Psychological science.

[31]  S. Carey,et al.  Whose gaze will infants follow? The elicitation of gaze-following in 12-month-olds , 1998 .

[32]  R. Baillargeon,et al.  Can a Self-Propelled Box Have a Goal? , 2005, Psychological science.

[33]  J. Mandler,et al.  Concept formation in infancy , 1993 .

[34]  D. Muir,et al.  Infant sensitivity to adult eye direction. , 1996, Child development.

[35]  Z. Nadasdy,et al.  Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age , 1995, Cognition.

[36]  D. Rakison,et al.  Developmental origin of the animate-inanimate distinction. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[37]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Infants' use of gaze direction to cue attention: The importance of perceived motion , 2000 .

[38]  R. Baillargeon,et al.  The Development of Young Infants' Intuitions about Support , 1992 .

[39]  P. D. Eimas,et al.  Evidence for a global categorical representation of humans by young infants. , 1998, Journal of experimental child psychology.