Rapid Processing of a Global Feature in the ON Visual Pathways of Behaving Monkeys

Visual objects are recognized by their features. Whereas, some features are based on simple components (i.e., local features, such as orientation of line segments), some features are based on the whole object (i.e., global features, such as an object having a hole in it). Over the past five decades, behavioral, physiological, anatomical, and computational studies have established a general model of vision, which starts from extracting local features in the lower visual pathways followed by a feature integration process that extracts global features in the higher visual pathways. This local-to-global model is successful in providing a unified account for a vast sets of perception experiments, but it fails to account for a set of experiments showing human visual systems' superior sensitivity to global features. Understanding the neural mechanisms underlying the “global-first” process will offer critical insights into new models of vision. The goal of the present study was to establish a non-human primate model of rapid processing of global features for elucidating the neural mechanisms underlying differential processing of global and local features. Monkeys were trained to make a saccade to a target in the black background, which was different from the distractors (white circle) in color (e.g., red circle target), local features (e.g., white square target), a global feature (e.g., white ring with a hole target) or their combinations (e.g., red square target). Contrary to the predictions of the prevailing local-to-global model, we found that (1) detecting a distinction or a change in the global feature was faster than detecting a distinction or a change in color or local features; (2) detecting a distinction in color was facilitated by a distinction in the global feature, but not in the local features; and (3) detecting the hole was interfered by the local features of the hole (e.g., white ring with a squared hole). These results suggest that monkey ON visual systems have a subsystem that is more sensitive to distinctions in the global feature than local features. They also provide the behavioral constraints for identifying the underlying neural substrates.

[1]  C. Kennard,et al.  Predictive eye saccades are different from visually triggered saccades , 1987, Vision Research.

[2]  Yan Zhuo,et al.  Contributions of the Visual Ventral Pathway to Long-Range Apparent Motion , 2003, Science.

[3]  S. Sutherland Eye, brain and vision , 1993, Nature.

[4]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  The differentiation of visually guided and anticipatory saccades in gap and overlap paradigms , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Saccade target selection in macaque during feature and conjunction visual search , 1999, Visual Neuroscience.

[6]  L. Chen,et al.  Holes and wholes: A reply to Rubin and Kanwisher , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[7]  Patrick Doherty,et al.  A Collaborative Framework for 3D Mapping Using Unmanned Aerial Vehicles , 2016, PRIMA.

[8]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  What neural pathways mediate express saccades? , 1993, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[9]  R. Desimone,et al.  Visual properties of neurons in a polysensory area in superior temporal sulcus of the macaque. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  Lin Chen,et al.  Reply Author's Response: Where to Begin? , 2022 .

[11]  Laurent Itti,et al.  Color-Related Signals in the Primate Superior Colliculus , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  L. Pessoa,et al.  Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a 'low road' to 'many roads' of evaluating biological significance , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[13]  Michele A. Basso,et al.  Modulation of neuronal activity by target uncertainty , 1997, Nature.

[14]  Julie H. Culp,et al.  Transformation of Receptive Field Properties from Lateral Geniculate Nucleus to Superficial V1 in the Tree Shrew , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  L Chen,et al.  Topological Structure in the Perception of Apparent Motion , 1985, Perception.

[16]  B. Richmond,et al.  Implantation of magnetic search coils for measurement of eye position: An improved method , 1980, Vision Research.

[17]  Juha Silvanto,et al.  Investigating the Causal Role of rOFA in Holistic Detection of Mooney Faces and Objects: An fMRI-guided TMS Study , 2016, Brain Stimulation.

[18]  A. Dale,et al.  Functional analysis of primary visual cortex (V1) in humans. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  D. Zoccolan Invariant visual object recognition and shape processing in rats , 2015, Behavioural Brain Research.

[20]  Yan Zhuo,et al.  Global topological dominance in the left hemisphere , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[21]  D. Navon Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception , 1977, Cognitive Psychology.

[22]  D. B. Bender,et al.  Visual properties of neurons in inferotemporal cortex of the Macaque. , 1972, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  D. Hubel,et al.  Shape and arrangement of columns in cat's striate cortex , 1963, The Journal of physiology.

[24]  J. Findlay,et al.  Express saccades: is there a separate population in humans? , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  R H Wurtz,et al.  Comparison of effects of eye movements and stimulus movements on striate cortex neurons of the monkey. , 1969, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  Wu Zhou,et al.  Premotor commands encode monocular eye movements , 1998, Nature.

[27]  B. Fischer,et al.  Separate populations of visually guided saccades in humans: reaction times and amplitudes , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  C. Bruce,et al.  Face recognition by monkeys: Absence of an inversion effect , 1982, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  Lin Chen,et al.  Topological change disturbs object continuity in attentive tracking , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[30]  Lin Chen The topological approach to perceptual organization , 2005 .

[31]  H. Barlow Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information: David Marr. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman, 1982. pp. xvi + 397 , 1983 .

[32]  H. Komatsu,et al.  Relationships between color, shape, and pattern selectivities of neurons in the inferior temporal cortex of the monkey. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  D P Munoz,et al.  Saccadic reaction time in the monkey: advanced preparation of oculomotor programs is primarily responsible for express saccade occurrence. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.

[35]  Zheng Shen,et al.  The hole precedence in face but not figure discrimination and its neuronal correlates , 2002, Vision Research.

[36]  D. Hubel,et al.  Ferrier lecture - Functional architecture of macaque monkey visual cortex , 1977, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[37]  D. Hubel,et al.  Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: anatomy, physiology, and perception. , 1988, Science.

[38]  D. Robinson,et al.  A METHOD OF MEASURING EYE MOVEMENT USING A SCLERAL SEARCH COIL IN A MAGNETIC FIELD. , 1963, IEEE transactions on bio-medical engineering.

[39]  M. A. Basso,et al.  Modulation of Neuronal Activity in Superior Colliculus by Changes in Target Probability , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  R. Desimone,et al.  Shape recognition and inferior temporal neurons. , 1983, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[41]  R. Desimone Face-Selective Cells in the Temporal Cortex of Monkeys , 1991, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  Lin Chen,et al.  Holes in illusory conjunctions , 1997 .

[43]  Lin Chen,et al.  Author's response: Where to begin? , 2005 .

[44]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[45]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Use of an extraretinal signal by monkey superior colliculus neurons to distinguish real from self-induced stimulus movement. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  Global perception in small brains: Topological pattern recognition in honey bees , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[47]  D. Hubel Exploration of the primary visual cortex, 1955–78 , 1982, Nature.

[48]  W. King,et al.  Attentional sensitivity and asymmetries of vertical saccade generation in monkey , 2002, Vision Research.

[49]  A. C. Smit,et al.  A short-latency transition in saccade dynamics during square-wave tracking and its significance for the differentiation of visually-guided and predictive saccades , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[50]  N Weisstein,et al.  Visual Detection of Line Segments: An Object-Superiority Effect , 1974, Science.

[51]  S. Hochstein,et al.  The reverse hierarchy theory of visual perceptual learning , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[52]  R. Shapley,et al.  “Black” Responses Dominate Macaque Primary Visual Cortex V1 , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  L Chen,et al.  Topological structure in visual perception. , 1982, Science.

[54]  Essa Yacoub,et al.  High-field fMRI unveils orientation columns in humans , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[55]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  Hierarchical organization and functional streams in the visual cortex , 1983, Trends in Neurosciences.