Moving One’s Own Body Part Induces a Motion Aftereffect Anchored to the Body Part

The question of how our body parts successfully interact with objects in the outside world is a fundamental problem in cognitive science and neuroscience. This problem is closely related to biologically important behaviors such as avoiding collisions or safely reaching for an object. Although previous studies have suggested that perceiving the space around one's own body is essential for interacting successfully with objects, how one's own body parts influence the ability to perceive the space around the body is unknown. Here, we report a visual motion aftereffect (MAE) that shows spatial selectivity in hand-centered coordinates. The MAE is an illusion of visual motion resulting from adaptation to a moving pattern and normally occurs with retinal overlap between adaptor and test. We found that the MAE occurs without retinal overlap between the adaptor and test when they are presented at the same position relative to a seen hand. This MAE appeared only when participants voluntarily controlled the hand that was felt to be their own. Our results reveal that sense of owning an actively moved body part generates a perceptual representation of the space encoded in body-part-centered coordinates that might be useful for guiding movements of one's body parts.

[1]  S. Nishida,et al.  Motion aftereffect with flickering test patterns reveals higher stages of motion processing , 1995, Vision Research.

[2]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  The motion aftereffect , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  C. Galletti,et al.  Role of the medial parieto-occipital cortex in the control of reaching and grasping movements , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  D. Knill,et al.  Kinesthetic information disambiguates visual motion signals , 2010, Current Biology.

[5]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  H. Ehrsson,et al.  Behavioural Brain Research , 1999 .

[7]  M. Graziano Where is my arm? The relative role of vision and proprioception in the neuronal representation of limb position. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[8]  M. Jeannerod The cognitive neuroscience of action , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[9]  F. Vignemont Embodiment, ownership and disownership , 2011, Consciousness and Cognition.

[10]  A. Marcel,et al.  The body and the self , 1995 .

[11]  C. Gross,et al.  Coding of visual space by premotor neurons. , 1994, Science.

[12]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Ventral intraparietal area of the macaque: anatomic location and visual response properties. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey , 1988, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  H. Ehrsson,et al.  Moving a Rubber Hand that Feels Like Your Own: A Dissociation of Ownership and Agency , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[15]  Giovanni Gentile,et al.  The Invisible Hand Illusion: Multisensory Integration Leads to the Embodiment of a Discrete Volume of Empty Space , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[16]  C. Moore,et al.  Movement and the Rubber Hand Illusion , 2009, Perception.

[17]  Dylan F. Cooke,et al.  Parieto-frontal interactions, personal space, and defensive behavior , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see , 1998, Nature.

[19]  K. Zilles,et al.  Polymodal Motion Processing in Posterior Parietal and Premotor Cortex A Human fMRI Study Strongly Implies Equivalencies between Humans and Monkeys , 2001, Neuron.

[20]  Y. Rossetti,et al.  Coding of Visual Space during Motor Preparation: Approaching Objects Rapidly Modulate Corticospinal Excitability in Hand-Centered Coordinates , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  M. Tsakiris My body in the brain: A neurocognitive model of body-ownership , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  C. Spence,et al.  Multisensory representation of limb position in human premotor cortex , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Motion adaptation shifts apparent position without the motion aftereffect , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[24]  Maria V. Sanchez-Vives,et al.  Inducing Illusory Ownership of a Virtual Body , 2009, Front. Neurosci..

[25]  David Burr,et al.  Spatiotopic perceptual maps in humans: evidence from motion adaptation , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[26]  Valeria I. Petkova,et al.  fMRI Adaptation Reveals a Cortical Mechanism for the Coding of Space Near the Hand , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  The reference frame of the motion aftereffect is retinotopic. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[28]  C. Gross,et al.  Visuospatial properties of ventral premotor cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  G. Mather,et al.  The motion aftereffect reloaded , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  Ehud Zohary,et al.  Is That Near My Hand? Multisensory Representation of Peripersonal Space in Human Intraparietal Sulcus , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  E. Holst Relations between the central Nervous System and the peripheral organs , 1954 .

[32]  H. Ehrsson,et al.  That's Near My Hand! Parietal and Premotor Coding of Hand-Centered Space Contributes to Localization and Self-Attribution of the Hand , 2012, Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  P. Haggard,et al.  Having a body versus moving your body: How agency structures body-ownership , 2006, Consciousness and Cognition.

[34]  J. Paillard The Body and the Self , 2002 .

[35]  A. Chaudhuri Modulation of the motion aftereffect by selective attention , 1990, Nature.

[36]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Sense of body and sense of action both contribute to self-recognition , 2002, Cognition.

[37]  R. Passingham,et al.  That's My Hand! Activity in Premotor Cortex Reflects Feeling of Ownership of a Limb , 2004, Science.

[38]  Hiroshi Watanabe,et al.  Interpretation of optic flows synchronized with observer’s hand movements , 2009, Vision Research.

[39]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Viewing the hand prior to movement improves accuracy of pointing performed toward the unseen contralateral hand , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  P. Haggard,et al.  The rubber hand illusion revisited: visuotactile integration and self-attribution. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[41]  G Rizzolatti,et al.  The Space Around Us , 1997, Science.