End or Means—The “What” and “How” of Observed Intentional Actions

Action understanding and learning are suggested to be mediated, at least in part, by the human mirror neuron system (hMNS). Static images as well as videos of actions with the outcome occluded have been shown to activate the hMNS. However, whether the hMNS preferentially responds to end or means of an action remains to be investigated. We, therefore, presented subjects with videos of intentional actions that were shown from two perspectives (factor 1, perspective: first vs. third person) while subjects directed their attention to the means or the end thereof (factor 2, task: means vs. end). End- or means-related changes in BOLD signal and corticospinal excitability (CSE) were assessed using fMRI and TMS, respectively. Judging the means of an action compared with its end differentially activated bilateral ventral premotor (vPMC) and inferior parietal cortex (IPL), that is, the core regions of the hMNS. The reverse contrast revealed left precuneus and bilateral superior frontal, angular, and middle temporal gyrus activity. In accordance, the two tasks, although identically in stimulus properties, modulated CSE differentially. Although recent studies suggest that the hMNS may prefer the presence of a goal or context, our data show that within the same context, it responds preferentially when attention is directed to the action means. Consequently, in addition to inferring action goals, a key function of the hMNS may be to anticipate the trajectories and dynamics of observed actions, which is a prerequisite for any timely interaction.

[1]  Leonardo Fogassi,et al.  Mirror Neurons Responding to Observation of Actions Made with Tools in Monkey Ventral Premotor Cortex , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[2]  Michael C. Corballis,et al.  FOXP2 and the mirror system , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  S. Wapner,et al.  Imitation of a model's hand movements: age changes in transposition of left-right relations. , 1968, Child development.

[4]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Parietal Lobe: From Action Organization to Intention Understanding , 2005, Science.

[5]  N. A. Borghese,et al.  Different Brain Correlates for Watching Real and Virtual Hand Actions , 2001, NeuroImage.

[6]  Michael A. Arbib,et al.  Synthetic brain imaging: grasping, mirror neurons and imitation , 2000, Neural Networks.

[7]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  J. Freyd,et al.  Transformations of visual memory induced by implied motions of pattern elements. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[9]  Sook-Lei Liew,et al.  Familiarity modulates mirror neuron and mentalizing regions during intention understanding , 2011, Human brain mapping.

[10]  S. Blakemore,et al.  Motor activation prior to observation of a predicted movement , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[11]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Lateralization of the Human Mirror Neuron System , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[13]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Modulation of premotor mirror neuron activity during observation of unpredictable grasping movements , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.

[14]  M. Iacoboni Neural mechanisms of imitation , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[15]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Formation of a Motor Memory by Action Observation , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  J. Gentle,et al.  Synchronized changes to relative neuron populations in postnatal human neocortical development , 2010, Cognitive Neurodynamics.

[17]  C. Frith,et al.  Interacting minds--a biological basis. , 1999, Science.

[18]  Julie Grèzes,et al.  Seeing what you can and can't do: action observation by experts studied with fMRI , 2003 .

[19]  P. Rossini,et al.  Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord and roots: basic principles and procedures for routine clinical application. Report of an IFCN committee. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[20]  R. Blake,et al.  Brain Areas Active during Visual Perception of Biological Motion , 2002, Neuron.

[21]  J. Rothwell,et al.  The physiological basis of transcranial motor cortex stimulation in conscious humans , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[22]  J. Decety,et al.  From the perception of action to the understanding of intention , 2001, Nature reviews. Neuroscience.

[23]  R. Johansson,et al.  Corticospinal control during reach, grasp, and precision lift in man , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[24]  T. Chartrand,et al.  The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[25]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Authorship effects in the prediction of handwriting strokes: Evidence for action simulation during action perception , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[26]  Marco Iacoboni,et al.  Lateralization in motor facilitation during action observation: a TMS study , 2001, NeuroImage.

[27]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[28]  F. Baldissera,et al.  Excitability changes in human corticospinal projections to muscles moving hand and fingers while viewing a reaching and grasping action , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[29]  Andrew N. Meltzoff,et al.  Neural circuits involved in imitation and perspective-taking , 2006, NeuroImage.

[30]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[31]  Scott H. Johnson-Frey What's So Special about Human Tool Use? , 2003, Neuron.

[32]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  How Many Subjects Constitute a Study? , 1999, NeuroImage.

[33]  C. Price,et al.  A functional neuroimaging study of the variables that generate category-specific object processing differences. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[34]  W. Schofield,et al.  Do Children Find Movements Which Cross the Body Midline Difficult? , 1976 .

[35]  T Shallice,et al.  'Theory of mind' and the prefrontal cortex. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[36]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Is human imitation based on a mirror-neurone system? Some behavioural evidence , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Children's coding of human action: cognitive factors influencing imitation in 3-year-olds. , 2000, Developmental science.

[38]  Dirk Kerzel,et al.  A matter of design: No representational momentum without predictability , 2002 .

[39]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Spatial registration and normalization of images , 1995 .

[40]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The functional anatomy of attention to visual motion. A functional MRI study. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[41]  R. Johansson,et al.  Prediction Precedes Control in Motor Learning , 2003, Current Biology.

[42]  Hiroshi Imamizu,et al.  Human cerebellar activity reflecting an acquired internal model of a new tool , 2000, Nature.

[43]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Does the End Justify the Means? A PET Exploration of the Mechanisms Involved in Human Imitation , 2002, NeuroImage.

[44]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Phase-specific modulation of cortical motor output during movement observation , 2001, Neuroreport.

[45]  Juha Silvanto,et al.  The causal role of category-specific neuronal representations in the left ventral premotor cortex (PMv) in semantic processing , 2010, NeuroImage.

[46]  T. Chartrand,et al.  Using Nonconscious Behavioral Mimicry to Create Affiliation and Rapport , 2003, Psychological science.

[47]  J. Decety,et al.  Top down effect of strategy on the perception of human biological motion: a pet investigation. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[48]  Uta Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.

[49]  M. Candidi,et al.  Mapping Implied Body Actions in the Human Motor System , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[50]  R. C. Oldfield THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .

[51]  Darren R Gitelman,et al.  ILAB: A program for postexperimental eye movement analysis , 2002, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[52]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[53]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The Cortical Motor System , 2001, Neuron.

[54]  M Jeannerod,et al.  The hand and the object: the role of posterior parietal cortex in forming motor representations. , 1994, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[55]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Modulation of motor and premotor activity during imitation of target-directed actions. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[56]  Michael A. Arbib,et al.  Mirror neurons and imitation: A computationally guided review , 2006, Neural Networks.

[57]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Identifying the What, Why, and How of an Observed Action: An fMRI Study of Mentalizing and Mechanizing during Action Observation , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[58]  M. Arbib From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[59]  L. Craighero,et al.  Human motor cortex excitability during the perception of others’ action , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[60]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[61]  D M Wolpert,et al.  Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control , 1998, Neural Networks.

[62]  L. Craighero,et al.  Modulation of spinal excitability during observation of hand actions in humans , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[63]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[64]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Motor facilitation following action observation: A behavioural study in prehensile action , 2003, Brain and Cognition.

[65]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  I Know What You Are Doing A Neurophysiological Study , 2001, Neuron.

[66]  Andrew A G Mattar,et al.  Motor Learning by Observing , 2005, Neuron.

[67]  R. E Passingham,et al.  Activations related to “mirror” and “canonical” neurones in the human brain: an fMRI study , 2003, NeuroImage.

[68]  Jens Haueisen,et al.  Involuntary Motor Activity in Pianists Evoked by Music Perception , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[69]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Grasping the Intentions of Others with One's Own Mirror Neuron System , 2005, PLoS biology.

[70]  J. Decety,et al.  Distinct Areas in Parietal Cortex Involved in Long-Term and Short-Term Action Planning: A Pet Investigation , 2002, Cortex.

[71]  T. Paus,et al.  Modulation of cortical excitability during action observation: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study , 2000, Neuroreport.

[72]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[73]  C. Heyes,et al.  Motor learning by observation: Evidence from a serial reaction time task , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[74]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Attentional modulation of effective connectivity from V2 to V5/MT in humans. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.