Parametric measurements of optic flow by humans

Motion pervades the visual world (Marr, 1982). This is so not only because images in nature are unlikely to be static, but also because motion constitutes a rich source of information to understand those images. The strategy that the brain uses to measure motion in images has been extensively studied and many theories have been proposed. The challenge has been to design a biologically plausible theory that will predict all motion phenomena and work on real images. With this approach, Yuille and Grzywacz (1998) have proposed a theoretical framework for visual motion that accounts for most existing psychophysical and physiological experiments. The theory proposes that the visual system fits internal models to the incoming retinal data, selecting the best models and their parameters. The fit begins with a measurement stage that performs local estimates of motion, such as local velocity (Bravo & Watamaniuk, 1995). These local estimates, which are noisy (Shadlen & Newsome, 1998) and sometimes ambiguous (Movshon et al., 1985), are then clustered (in a space of measurement variables) into regions whose boundaries correspond to motion boundaries. This clustering is performed by a number of competitive processes corresponding to different motions. The theory proposes that different types of tests can compete to perform this clustering. For example, the grouping can be done by either parametric or non-parametric tests. The former test will try to detect familiar motions defined by prior statistics-of-natural-scenes models, while the latter will allow the visual system to deal with general types of motion that may never have been seen before.

[1]  D. Burr,et al.  Two stages of visual processing for radial and circular motion , 1995, Nature.

[2]  S. McKee,et al.  Predicting future motion. , 2002, Journal of vision.

[3]  T. Radil,et al.  Selective directional sensitivity in visual motion perception , 1991, Vision Research.

[4]  D. Regan,et al.  Dissociation of discrimination thresholds for time to contact and for rate of angular expansion , 1993, Vision Research.

[5]  A Johnston,et al.  Concurrent measurement of perceived speed and speed discrimination threshold using the method of single stimuli , 1999, Vision Research.

[6]  J J Koenderink,et al.  Visual Size Invariance Does Not Apply to Geometric Angle and Speed of Rotation , 1993, Perception.

[7]  T. Freeman,et al.  Human sensitivity to expanding and rotating motion: effects of complementary masking and directional structure , 1992, Vision Research.

[8]  C. Clifford,et al.  The perception and discrimination of speed in complex motion , 1999, Vision Research.

[9]  Takeo Watanabe,et al.  High-Level Motion Processing , 1998 .

[10]  V. S. Ramachandran,et al.  Extrapolation of motion path in human visual perception , 1983, Vision Research.

[11]  A. Verri,et al.  First-order analysis of optical flow in monkey brain. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[12]  E. Adelson,et al.  The analysis of moving visual patterns , 1985 .

[13]  K. Tanaka,et al.  Analysis of motion of the visual field by direction, expansion/contraction, and rotation cells clustered in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque monkey. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  G. Orban,et al.  Responses of macaque STS neurons to optic flow components: a comparison of areas MT and MST. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Sensitivity of MST neurons to optic flow stimuli. II. Mechanisms of response selectivity revealed by small-field stimuli. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  S. McKee,et al.  Precise velocity discrimination despite random variations in temporal frequency and contrast , 1986, Vision Research.

[17]  Norberto M Grzywacz,et al.  Measurement of angular velocity in the perception of rotation , 2002, Vision Research.

[18]  G. Orban,et al.  Human velocity and direction discrimination measured with random dot patterns , 1988, Vision Research.

[19]  Charles G. Gross,et al.  Pattern recognition mechanisms , 1985 .

[20]  K. Tanaka,et al.  Underlying mechanisms of the response specificity of expansion/contraction and rotation cells in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque monkey. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  W. Newsome,et al.  The Variable Discharge of Cortical Neurons: Implications for Connectivity, Computation, and Information Coding , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Sensitivity of MST neurons to optic flow stimuli. I. A continuum of response selectivity to large-field stimuli. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  D. Burr,et al.  Seeing objects in motion , 1986, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[24]  S. Watamaniuk,et al.  Visible Persistence is Reduced by Fixed-Trajectory Motion but Not by Random Motion , 1992, Perception.

[25]  N. Grzywacz,et al.  Temporal coherence in visual rotation , 2002, Vision Research.

[26]  S. McKee A local mechanism for differential velocity detection , 1981, Vision Research.

[27]  Alan L. Yuille,et al.  Probabilistic Motion Estimation Based on Temporal Coherence , 2000, Neural Computation.

[28]  D C Van Essen,et al.  Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  Scott N. J. Watamaniuk,et al.  Seeing motion behind occluders , 1995, Nature.

[30]  M. Graziano,et al.  Tuning of MST neurons to spiral motions , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[31]  S. McKee,et al.  Temporal coherence theory for the detection and measurement of visual motion , 1995, Vision Research.

[32]  Robert J. Snowden,et al.  The Effects of Adapting to Complex Motions: Position Invariance and Tuning to Spiral Motions , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  D Regan,et al.  Motion in depth: Adequate and inadequate simulation , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[34]  Jan J. Koenderink,et al.  Local structure of movement parallax of the plane , 1976 .

[35]  N. Qian,et al.  A Novel Speed Illusion Involving Expansion and Rotation Patterns , 1996, Vision Research.

[36]  D Regan,et al.  Visual responses to vorticity and the neural analysis of optic flow. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[37]  S. McKee,et al.  Detecting a trajectory embedded in random-direction motion noise , 1995, Vision Research.

[38]  J. Berger Statistical Decision Theory and Bayesian Analysis , 1988 .

[39]  Norberto M Grzywacz,et al.  Local computation of angular velocity in rotational visual motion. , 2003, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[40]  David Marr,et al.  VISION A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information , 2009 .

[41]  Mary J. Bravo,et al.  Evidence for two speed signals: a coarse local signal for segregation and a precise global signal for discrimination , 1995, Vision Research.

[42]  Ning Qian,et al.  The effect of complex motion pattern on speed perception , 1998, Vision Research.