Spatial Attention Changes Excitability of Human Visual Cortex to Direct Stimulation

Summary Conscious perception depends not only on sensory input, but also on attention 1, 2. Recent studies in monkeys 3, 4, 5, 6 and humans 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12 suggest that influences of spatial attention on visual awareness may reflect top-down influences on excitability of visual cortex. Here we tested this specifically, by providing direct input into human visual cortex via cortical transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to produce illusory visual percepts, called phosphenes. We found that a lower TMS intensity was needed to elicit a conscious phosphene when its apparent spatial location was attended, rather than unattended. Our results indicate that spatial attention can enhance visual-cortex excitability, and visual awareness, even when sensory signals from the eye via the thalamic pathway are bypassed.

[1]  D N Rushton,et al.  The extent of the region of occipital cortex that when stimulated gives phosphenes fixed in the visual field. , 1972, The Journal of physiology.

[2]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Increased Activity in Human Visual Cortex during Directed Attention in the Absence of Visual Stimulation , 1999, Neuron.

[3]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Determinants and mechanisms of attentional modulation of neural processing. , 2001, Frontiers in Bioscience.

[4]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[5]  G A Orban,et al.  Attention-dependent suppression of metabolic activity in the early stages of the macaque visual system. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  B. Fischer,et al.  Separate populations of visually guided saccades in humans: reaction times and amplitudes , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  F A Wichmann,et al.  Ning for Helpful Comments and Suggestions. This Paper Benefited Con- Siderably from Conscientious Peer Review, and We Thank Our Reviewers the Psychometric Function: I. Fitting, Sampling, and Goodness of Fit , 2001 .

[8]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Fast Backprojections from the Motion to the Primary Visual Area Necessary for Visual Awareness , 2001, Science.

[9]  Antigona Martínez,et al.  Source analysis of event-related cortical activity during visuo-spatial attention. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[10]  K. Welch,et al.  Brain excitability in migraine: evidence from transcranial magnetic stimulation studies. , 1998, Current opinion in neurology.

[11]  A. Dale,et al.  The Retinotopy of Visual Spatial Attention , 1998, Neuron.

[12]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The physiological basis of attentional modulation in extrastriate visual areas , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Study and modulation of human cortical excitability with transcranial magnetic stimulation. , 1998, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[14]  C. Schroeder,et al.  Intermodal selective attention in monkeys. I: distribution and timing of effects across visual areas. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  M. Pinsk,et al.  Attention modulates responses in the human lateral geniculate nucleus , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  Lamme Vaf,et al.  Why visual attention and awareness are different , 2003 .

[17]  Sven Bestmann,et al.  Phosphene threshold as a function of contrast of external visual stimuli , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  V. Amassian,et al.  Suppression of visual perception by magnetic coil stimulation of human occipital cortex. , 1989, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[19]  E. Viding,et al.  Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[20]  Burkhart Fischer,et al.  Saccadic eye movements of dyslexic adult subjects , 1993, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  S J Luck,et al.  Visual event-related potentials index focused attention within bilateral stimulus arrays. II. Functional dissociation of P1 and N1 components. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[22]  L. Cohen,et al.  Reduction of human visual cortex excitability using 1-Hz transcranial magnetic stimulation , 2000, Neurology.

[23]  David W. Royal,et al.  On the impact of attention and motor planning on the lateral geniculate nucleus. , 2005, Progress in brain research.

[24]  E Corthout,et al.  Timing of activity in early visual cortex as revealed by transcranial magnetic stimulation. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[25]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Involvement of striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in spatial attention , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[26]  Brindley Gs,et al.  The extent of the region of occipital cortex that when stimulated gives phosphenes fixed in the visual field. , 1972 .

[27]  D. Heeger,et al.  Activity in primary visual cortex predicts performance in a visual detection task , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[28]  R. Reid,et al.  Attention Modulates the Responses of Simple Cells in Monkey Primary Visual Cortex , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Junying Yuan,et al.  Selective gating of visual signals by microstimulation of frontal cortex , 2022 .

[30]  R W Guillery,et al.  The role of the thalamus in the flow of information to the cortex. , 2002, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[31]  S. Sherman,et al.  Thalamic relays and cortical functioning. , 2005, Progress in brain research.

[32]  G. Brindley,et al.  The sensations produced by electrical stimulation of the visual cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[33]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[34]  D. Heeger,et al.  Spatial attention affects brain activity in human primary visual cortex. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[35]  Darren R Gitelman,et al.  ILAB: A program for postexperimental eye movement analysis , 2002, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[36]  S J Luck,et al.  Visual event-related potentials index focused attention within bilateral stimulus arrays. I. Evidence for early selection. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[37]  C. Frith,et al.  Shifting baselines in attention research , 2000, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[38]  B. C. Motter Focal attention produces spatially selective processing in visual cortical areas V1, V2, and V4 in the presence of competing stimuli. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  A. Cowey,et al.  Magnetically induced phosphenes in sighted, blind and blindsighted observers , 2000, Neuroreport.

[40]  C. Blakemore,et al.  The site of saccadic suppression , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[41]  M. Erb,et al.  The influence of current direction on phosphene thresholds evoked by transcranial magnetic stimulation , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[42]  George R. Mangun,et al.  Sustained visual-spatial attention produces costs and benefits in response time and evoked neural activity , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  Tony Ro,et al.  Feedback Contributions to Visual Awareness in Human Occipital Cortex , 2003, Current Biology.

[44]  Richard A. Tyrrell,et al.  A rapid technique to assess the resting states of the eyes and other threshold phenomena: The Modified Binary Search (MOBS) , 1988 .