Young children's analogical problem solving: gaining insights from video displays.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] R. Siegler. Microgenetic Analyses of Learning , 2007 .
[2] G. Halford. Children's Understanding: The Development of Mental Models , 1993 .
[3] J. Sommerville,et al. Experience matters: the impact of doing versus watching on infants' subsequent perception of tool-use events. , 2008, Developmental psychology.
[4] K. Holyoak,et al. Development of analogical problem-solving skill. , 1984, Child development.
[5] Patricia J. Bauer,et al. Episodic Memory in 16- and 20-Month-Old Children: Specifics Are Generalized but Not Forgotten. , 1994 .
[6] Michael Tomasello,et al. The Cultural Ecology of Young Children’s Interactions With Objects and Artifacts , 1999 .
[7] J. Sommerville,et al. Age-related changes in children's use of external representations. , 1999, Developmental psychology.
[8] Zhe Chen,et al. From beyond to within their grasp: the rudiments of analogical problem solving in 10- and 13-month-olds. , 1997, Developmental psychology.
[9] D. Klahr,et al. All other things being equal: acquisition and transfer of the control of variables strategy. , 1999, Child development.
[10] A. Meltzoff. Immediate and Deferred Imitation in Fourteen- and Twenty-Four-Month-Old Infants. , 1985 .
[11] Usha Goswami,et al. Transitive Relational Mappings in Three‐ and Four‐Year‐Olds: The Analogy of Goldilocks and the Three Bears , 1995 .
[12] Z. Chen,et al. Children's analogical problem solving: the effects of superficial, structural, and procedural similarity. , 1996, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[13] Rachel Barr,et al. Transfer of learning between 2D and 3D sources during infancy: Informing theory and practice. , 2010, Developmental review : DR.
[14] Georgene L. Troseth,et al. TV guide: two-year-old children learn to use video as a source of information. , 2003, Developmental psychology.
[15] Erika Tunteler,et al. Effects of prior assistance in using analogies on young children's unprompted analogical problem solving over time: a microgenetic study. , 2007, The British journal of educational psychology.
[16] Thomas Suddendorf,et al. Visual self-recognition in mirrors and live videos: Evidence for a developmental asynchrony , 2007 .
[17] A. Meltzoff. Imitation of televised models by infants. , 1988, Child development.
[18] H. Hayne,et al. Imitation from television by 24- and 30-month-olds. , 2003 .
[19] Mary Jo Kane,et al. Young children's mental models determine analogical transfer across problems with a common goal structure * , 1986 .
[20] Philippe Rochat,et al. Learning from others in 9‐18‐month‐old infants , 2006 .
[21] Georgene L. Troseth,et al. Young children's use of video as a source of socially relevant information. , 2006, Child development.
[22] Dedre Gentner,et al. Systematicity and Surface Similarity in the Development of Analogy , 1986, Cogn. Sci..
[23] Ann L. Brown. Analogical learning and transfer: what develops? , 1989 .
[24] H. Hayne,et al. Developmental changes in imitation from television during infancy. , 1999, Child development.
[25] Jessica A. Sommerville,et al. Infants' Sensitivity to the Causal Features of Means‐End Support Sequences in Action and Perception , 2005 .
[26] Daniel R. Anderson,et al. Television and Reality: Toddlers' Use of Visual Information from Video to Guide Behavior , 2002 .
[27] A. Woodward,et al. The goal trumps the means: Highlighting goals is more beneficial than highlighting means in means-end training. , 2013, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[28] Andrew N Meltzoff,et al. Peer Imitation by Toddlers in Laboratory, Home, and Day-Care Contexts: Implications for Social Learning and Memory. , 1993, Developmental psychology.
[29] Z Chen,et al. Intention and outcome: key components of causal structure facilitating mapping in children's analogical transfer. , 1992, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[30] Kenneth D. Forbus,et al. The Roles of Similarity in Transfer: Separating Retrievability From Inferential Soundness , 1993, Cognitive Psychology.
[31] Andrew N Meltzoff,et al. Deferred Imitation Across Changes in Context and Object: Memory and Generalization in 14-Month-Old Infants. , 1996, Infant behavior & development.
[32] Georgene L. Troseth,et al. Do Babies Learn From Baby Media? , 2010, Psychological science.
[33] R. Siegler,et al. The rebirth of children's learning. , 2000, Child development.
[34] Rachel Barr,et al. Age-related changes in deferred imitation from television by 6- to 18-month-olds. , 2007, Developmental science.
[35] Ann L. Brown,et al. Inducing Flexible Thinking: The Problem of Access , 1981 .
[36] Zhe Chen,et al. Worth One Thousand Words: Children's Use of Pictures in Analogical Problem Solving , 2003 .
[37] D. Gentner,et al. Systematicity and Surface Similarity in the Development of Analogy. Technical Report No. 358. , 1985 .
[38] R. Siegler,et al. How Does Change Occur: A Microgenetic Study of Number Conservation , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.
[39] Judy S. DeLoache,et al. Early understanding of the representational function of pictures , 1994, Cognition.
[40] R. Siegler,et al. ACROSS THE GREAT DIVIDE : BRIDGING THE GAP BETWEEN UNDERSTANDING OF TODDLERS ' AND OLDER CHILDREN ' S THINKING Zhe , 2002 .
[41] Harlene Hayne,et al. Developmental changes in the specificity of memory over the second year of life , 1997 .
[42] Jean M. Mandler,et al. Putting the horse before the cart: The use of temporal order in recall of events by one-year-old children. , 1992 .
[43] J. Deloache,et al. The medium can obscure the message: young children's understanding of video. , 1998, Child development.