Mind the Gap: Olfactory Trace Conditioning in Honeybees

Trace conditioning is a form of classical conditioning, where a neutral stimulus (conditioned stimulus, CS) is associated with a following appetitive or aversive stimulus (unconditioned stimulus, US). Unlike classical delay conditioning, in trace conditioning there is a stimulus-free gap between CS and US, and thus a poststimulus neural representation (trace) of the CS is required to bridge the gap until its association with the US. The properties of such stimulus traces are not well understood, nor are their underlying physiological mechanisms. Using behavioral and physiological approaches, we studied appetitive olfactory trace conditioning in honeybees. We found that single-odor presentation created a trace containing information about odor identity. This trace conveyed odor information about the initial stimulus and was robust against interference by other odors. Memory acquisition decreased with increasing CS–US gap length. The maximum learnable CS–US gap length could be extended by previous trace-conditioning experience. Furthermore, acquisition improved when an additional odor was presented during the CS–US gap. Using calcium imaging, we tested whether projection neurons in the primary olfactory brain area, the antennal lobe, contain a CS trace. We found odor-specific persistent responses after stimulus offset. These post-odor responses, however, did not encode the CS trace, and perceived odor quality could be predicted by the initial but not by the post-odor response. Our data suggest that olfactory trace conditioning is a less reflexive form of learning than classical delay conditioning, indicating that odor traces might involve higher-level cognitive processes.

[1]  K. Mori,et al.  The olfactory bulb: coding and processing of odor molecule information. , 1999, Science.

[2]  Andreas S. Thum,et al.  Smelling, tasting, learning: Drosophila as a study case. , 2009, Results and problems in cell differentiation.

[3]  R. Menzel,et al.  The glomerular code for odor representation is species specific in the honeybee Apis mellifera , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[4]  R. Menzel,et al.  Odorant intensity as a determinant for olfactory conditioning in honeybees: roles in discrimination, overshadowing and memory consolidation. , 1997, The Journal of experimental biology.

[5]  M. Giurfa,et al.  Perceptual and Neural Olfactory Similarity in Honeybees , 2005, PLoS biology.

[6]  M. Heisenberg Mushroom body memoir: from maps to models , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[7]  W. B. Orr,et al.  Hippocampectomy selectively disrupts discrimination reversal conditioning of the rabbit nictitating membrane response , 1983, Behavioural Brain Research.

[8]  P. I. Pavlov Conditioned reflexes: An investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex. , 1929, Annals of Neurosciences.

[9]  R. Menzel Working Memory in Bees: Also in Flies? , 2009, Journal of neurogenetics.

[10]  C Giovanni Galizia,et al.  Temporal integrity of an airborne odor stimulus is greatly affected by physical aspects of the odor delivery system. , 2006, Chemical senses.

[11]  Randolf Menzel,et al.  Sensory Memory for Odors Is Encoded in Spontaneous Correlated Activity Between Olfactory Glomeruli , 2006, Neural Computation.

[12]  R. Menzel,et al.  Associative and Non-Associative Plasticity in Kenyon Cells of the Honeybee Mushroom Body , 2008, Frontiers in systems neuroscience.

[13]  M. Stengl,et al.  Octopamine and tyramine modulate pheromone-sensitive olfactory sensilla of the hawkmoth Manduca sexta in a time-dependent manner , 2009, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[14]  U. Müller Learning in honeybees: from molecules to behaviour. , 2002, Zoology.

[15]  G. H. Lunney,et al.  USING ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE WITH A DICHOTOMOUS DEPENDENT VARIABLE: AN EMPIRICAL STUDY , 1970 .

[16]  C. Galizia,et al.  Olfactory coding in the insect brain: molecular receptive ranges, spatial and temporal coding , 2008 .

[17]  J. Desmond,et al.  Neural substrates underlying human delay and trace eyeblink conditioning , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[18]  M. Hammer An identified neuron mediates the unconditioned stimulus in associative olfactory learning in honeybees , 1993, Nature.

[19]  C Giovanni Galizia,et al.  Computational modeling suggests that response properties rather than spatial position determine connectivity between olfactory glomeruli. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  R. Menzel Searching for the memory trace in a mini-brain, the honeybee. , 2001, Learning & memory.

[21]  S. Sachse,et al.  Role of inhibition for temporal and spatial odor representation in olfactory output neurons: a calcium imaging study. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  R. Menzel,et al.  Odor coding in projection neurons of the honeybee brain. , 2005, Chemical senses.

[23]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  The concepts of ‘sameness’ and ‘difference’ in an insect , 2001, Nature.

[24]  R. Menzel,et al.  A digital three-dimensional atlas of the honeybee antennal lobe based on optical sections acquired by confocal microscopy , 1999, Cell and Tissue Research.

[25]  Ronald L. Davis,et al.  Dynamics of Learning-Related cAMP Signaling and Stimulus Integration in the Drosophila Olfactory Pathway , 2009, Neuron.

[26]  B. Smith,et al.  A honeybee's ability to learn, recognize, and discriminate odors depends upon odor sampling time and concentration. , 2009, Behavioral neuroscience.

[27]  Masutaro Kuwabara Bildung des bedingten Reflexes von Pavlovs Typus bei der Honigbiene, Apis mellifica (Mit 1 Textabbildung) , 1957 .

[28]  Richard F. Thompson,et al.  Neural substrates of eyeblink conditioning: acquisition and retention. , 2003, Learning & memory.

[29]  R. Clark,et al.  Classical conditioning and brain systems: the role of awareness. , 1998, Science.

[30]  I. Rodriguez,et al.  Pheromone sensing in mice. , 2009, Results and problems in cell differentiation.

[31]  J. Disterhoft,et al.  Where is the trace in trace conditioning? , 2008, Trends in Neurosciences.

[32]  Mandyam V Srinivasan,et al.  Visual working memory in decision making by honey bees. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[33]  W. F. Prokasy,et al.  Classical eyelid conditioning as a function of sustained and shifted interstimulus intervals , 1963 .

[34]  R. Menzel,et al.  Appetitive odor learning does not change olfactory coding in a subpopulation of honeybee antennal lobe neurons , 2006, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[35]  Michael E Hasselmo,et al.  Behavioral Responses to Aliphatic Aldehydes Can Be Predicted From Known Electrophysiological Responses of Mitral Cells in the Olfactory Bulb , 1999, Physiology & Behavior.

[36]  Elliot A. Ludvig,et al.  Magnitude and timing of conditioned responses in delay and trace classical conditioning of the nictitating membrane response of the rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus). , 2009, Behavioral neuroscience.

[37]  P. Kaplan,et al.  Bridging temporal gaps between CS and US in autoshaping: A test of a local context hypothesis , 1984 .

[38]  M. D’Esposito Working memory. , 2008, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[39]  M. Giurfa,et al.  Using local anaesthetics to block neuronal activity and map specific learning tasks to the mushroom bodies of an insect brain , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[40]  A. Kanellopoulos,et al.  Protection from premature habituation requires functional mushroom bodies in Drosophila. , 2007, Learning & memory.

[41]  T. Shors,et al.  Trace Conditioning and the Hippocampus: The Importance of Contiguity , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  Silke Sachse,et al.  The coding of odour‐intensity in the honeybee antennal lobe: local computation optimizes odour representation , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[43]  R. Rescorla,et al.  A theory of Pavlovian conditioning : Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement , 1972 .

[44]  Paul Szyszka,et al.  Olfactory Trace Conditioning in Drosophila , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  C. Giovanni Galizia,et al.  Odor-Driven Attractor Dynamics in the Antennal Lobe Allow for Simple and Rapid Olfactory Pattern Classification , 2004, Neural Computation.

[46]  I. Pavlov,et al.  Conditioned reflexes: An investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex. , 1929, Annals of neurosciences.

[47]  R. Clark Eyeblink conditioning and systems consolidation: An ironic yet powerful pairing , 2011, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[48]  G. Shepherd,et al.  Mechanisms of olfactory discrimination: converging evidence for common principles across phyla. , 1997, Annual review of neuroscience.

[49]  B. Raman,et al.  Sparse odor representation and olfactory learning , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[50]  V. Rehder Quantification of the honeybee's proboscis reflex by electromyographic recordings , 1987 .

[51]  Louis D. Matzel,et al.  The Role of the Hippocampus in Trace Conditioning: Temporal Discontinuity or Task Difficulty? , 2001, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[52]  Peter Dayan,et al.  A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward , 1997, Science.

[53]  M. Giurfa Behavioral and neural analysis of associative learning in the honeybee: a taste from the magic well , 2007, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[54]  R. Menzel,et al.  Sparsening and temporal sharpening of olfactory representations in the honeybee mushroom bodies. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[55]  M. Bitterman,et al.  Classical conditioning of proboscis extension in honeybees (Apis mellifera). , 1983, Journal of comparative psychology.

[56]  Glenn C. Turner,et al.  Oscillations and Sparsening of Odor Representations in the Mushroom Body , 2002, Science.

[57]  J. Disterhoft,et al.  Sequence of single neuron changes in CA1 hippocampus of rabbits during acquisition of trace eyeblink conditioned responses. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[58]  E A Wasserman,et al.  Trace autoshaping: Acquisition, maintenance, and path dependence at long trace intervals. , 1981, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[59]  M. Bitterman,et al.  Learning by Honeybees in an Unnatural Situation , 1983 .

[60]  S. Sachse,et al.  The spatial representation of chemical structures in the antennal lobe of honeybees: steps towards the olfactory code , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.

[61]  Glenn C. Turner,et al.  Olfactory representations by Drosophila mushroom body neurons. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[62]  R. F. Thompson,et al.  Hippocampus and trace conditioning of the rabbit's classically conditioned nictitating membrane response. , 1986, Behavioral neuroscience.

[63]  D. Shanks,et al.  The role of awareness in Pavlovian conditioning: empirical evidence and theoretical implications. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.