Limb position drift: implications for control of posture and movement.

In the absence of visual feedback, subject reports of hand location tend to drift over time. Such drift has been attributed to a gradual reduction in the usefulness of proprioception to signal limb position. If this account is correct, drift should degrade the accuracy of movement distance and direction over a series of movements made without visual feedback. To test this hypothesis, we asked participants to perform six series of 75 repetitive movements from a visible start location to a visible target, in time with a regular, audible tone. Fingertip position feedback was given by a cursor during the first five trials in the series. Feedback was then removed, and participants were to continue on pace for the next 70 trials. Movements were made in two directions (30 degrees and 120 degrees ) from each of three start locations (initial shoulder angles of 30 degrees, 40 degrees, 50 degrees, and initial elbow angles of 90 degrees ). Over the 70 trials, the start location of each movement drifted, on average, 8 cm away from the initial start location. This drift varied systematically with movement direction, indicating that drift is related to movement production. However, despite these dramatic changes in hand position and joint configuration, movement distance and direction remained relatively constant. Inverse dynamics analysis revealed that movement preservation was accompanied by substantial modification of joint muscle torque. These results suggest that proprioception continues to be a reliable source of limb position information after prolonged time without vision, but that this information is used differently for maintaining limb position and for specifying movement trajectory.

[1]  B. C. Abbott,et al.  The relation between velocity of shortening and the tension‐length curve of skeletal muscle , 1953, The Journal of physiology.

[2]  D. Wilkie The mechanical properties of muscle. , 1956, British medical bulletin.

[3]  J. Paillard,et al.  Active and passive movements in the calibration of position sense , 1968 .

[4]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  E A Roy,et al.  The codability of kinesthetic location and distance information. , 1972, Acta psychologica.

[6]  M. McGlamery Mammalian Muscle Receptors and Their Central Actions , 1973 .

[7]  D. McCloskey,et al.  Detections of movements imposed on finger, elbow and shoulder joints. , 1983, The Journal of physiology.

[8]  M. Smyth,et al.  4 – Memory for Movements , 1984 .

[9]  F. J. Clark,et al.  Role of intramuscular receptors in the awareness of limb position. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Neuronal population coding of movement direction. , 1986, Science.

[11]  F. Zajac Muscle and tendon: properties, models, scaling, and application to biomechanics and motor control. , 1989, Critical reviews in biomedical engineering.

[12]  Paul B. Johnson,et al.  Making arm movements within different parts of space: dynamic aspects in the primate motor cortex , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[13]  J. L. Taylor,et al.  Ability to detect angular displacements of the fingers made at an imperceptibly slow speed. , 1990, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[14]  R Caminiti,et al.  Making arm movements within different parts of space: the premotor and motor cortical representation of a coordinate system for reaching to visual targets , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  Nicholas I. Fisher,et al.  Statistical Analysis of Circular Data , 1993 .

[16]  C. Ghez,et al.  Loss of proprioception produces deficits in interjoint coordination. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  J. Lackner,et al.  Rapid adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of arm trajectory. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  J. Gordon,et al.  Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. II. Effects of visual information on accuracy. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  R L Sainburg,et al.  Control of limb dynamics in normal subjects and patients without proprioception. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  J. Gordon,et al.  Learning a visuomotor transformation in a local area of work space produces directional biases in other areas. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  J. Gordon,et al.  Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. I. Spatial errors. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Vectorial coding of movement: vision, proprioception, or both? , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  J. Lackner,et al.  Motor adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of reaching movements: endpoint but not trajectory adaptation transfers to the nonexposed arm. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  H N Zelaznik,et al.  The role of vision in repetitive circle drawing. , 1996, Acta psychologica.

[25]  G. M. Redding,et al.  Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  S. Scott,et al.  Reaching movements with similar hand paths but different arm orientations. I. Activity of individual cells in motor cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Viewing the hand prior to movement improves accuracy of pointing performed toward the unseen contralateral hand , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  S. Scott,et al.  Reaching movements with similar hand paths but different arm orientations. II. Activity of individual cells in dorsal premotor cortex and parietal area 5. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  P. Viviani,et al.  Pointing errors reflect biases in the perception of the initial hand position. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Maintaining internal representations: the role of the human superior parietal lobe , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  P. Viviani,et al.  Frames of reference and control parameters in visuomanual pointing. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  Anne C. Sittig,et al.  The precision of proprioceptive position sense , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  R. J. van Beers,et al.  Integration of proprioceptive and visual position-information: An experimentally supported model. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  M. Graziano Where is my arm? The relative role of vision and proprioception in the neuronal representation of limb position. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[35]  Stephan P. Swinnen,et al.  Proprioceptive control of multijoint movement: unimanual circle drawing , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  Stephan P. Swinnen,et al.  Proprioceptive control of multijoint movement: bimanual circle drawing , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  David A. Rosenbaum,et al.  Remembered positions: stored locations or stored postures? , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  D J Ostry,et al.  Compensation for interaction torques during single- and multijoint limb movement. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  R L Sainburg,et al.  Intersegmental dynamics are controlled by sequential anticipatory, error correction, and postural mechanisms. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  J. Lackner,et al.  Aspects of body self-calibration , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[41]  Michel Desmurget,et al.  Proprioception does not quickly drift during visual occlusion , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  R. Sainburg,et al.  Differences in control of limb dynamics during dominant and nondominant arm reaching. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  J. Lackner,et al.  Coriolis-force-induced trajectory and endpoint deviations in the reaching movements of labyrinthine-defective subjects. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  D. Rosenbaum,et al.  Posture-based motion planning: applications to grasping. , 2001, Psychological review.

[45]  R. Sainburg,et al.  Interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations: independence of direction and final position information , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[46]  Robert L Sainburg,et al.  Handedness: dominant arm advantages in control of limb dynamics. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  David A. Rosenbaum,et al.  Movement speed effects on limb position drift , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[48]  R. Sainburg,et al.  Differential contributions of vision and proprioception to movement accuracy , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[49]  M. Latash,et al.  Effects of altering initial position on movement direction and extent. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[50]  Optimal response of eye and hand motor systems in pointing at a visual target , 1979, Biological Cybernetics.

[51]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Optimal response of eye and hand motor systems in pointing at a visual target , 1979, Biological Cybernetics.

[52]  J. Wann,et al.  Does limb proprioception drift? , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[53]  D. M. MacKay,et al.  Source density analysis of scalp potentials during linguistic and non-linguistic processing of visual stimuli , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  O. Bock,et al.  Goal-directed arm movements in absence of visual guidance: evidence for amplitude rather than position control , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[55]  J. Gordon,et al.  Accuracy of planar reaching movements , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[56]  C. C. A. M. Gielen,et al.  The attainment of target position during step-tracking movements despite a shift of initial position , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[57]  O. Bock,et al.  Error accumulation and error correction in sequential pointing movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.