The anticipation of events in time

Humans anticipate events signaled by sensory cues. It is commonly assumed that two uncertainty parameters modulate the brain's capacity to predict: the hazard rate (HR) of event probability and the uncertainty in time estimation which increases with elapsed time. We investigate both assumptions by presenting event probability density functions (PDFs) in each of three sensory modalities. We show that perceptual systems use the reciprocal PDF and not the HR to model event probability density. We also demonstrate that temporal uncertainty does not necessarily grow with elapsed time but can also diminish, depending on the event PDF. Previous research identified neuronal activity related to event probability in multiple levels of the cortical hierarchy (sensory (V4), association (LIP), motor and other areas) proposing the HR as an elementary neuronal computation. Our results—consistent across vision, audition, and somatosensation—suggest that the neurobiological implementation of event anticipation is based on a different, simpler and more stable computation than HR: the reciprocal PDF of events in time. The brain predicts upcoming events—a fundamental operation assumed to depend on the event hazard rate and a linearly increasing uncertainty in time estimation. Here, the authors propose a simpler computation based on the reciprocal PDF, which directly determines the uncertainty in time estimation.

[1]  James V. Stone Information Theory: A Tutorial Introduction , 2015, ArXiv.

[2]  S. Gershman,et al.  Dopamine reward prediction errors reflect hidden state inference across time , 2017, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  M. Shadlen,et al.  A representation of the hazard rate of elapsed time in macaque area LIP , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[4]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  Spatial Attention and Temporal Expectation Under Timed Uncertainty Predictably Modulate Neuronal Responses in Monkey V1. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[5]  S. Gershman,et al.  The Medial Prefrontal Cortex Shapes Dopamine Reward Prediction Errors under State Uncertainty , 2018, Neuron.

[6]  J. Schoffelen,et al.  Neuronal Coherence as a Mechanism of Effective Corticospinal Interaction , 2005, Science.

[7]  D. Buonomano,et al.  The neural basis of temporal processing. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[8]  B. Libet,et al.  Responses of Human Somatosensory Cortex to Stimuli below Threshold for Conscious Sensation , 1967, Science.

[9]  W. Newsome,et al.  The temporal precision of reward prediction in dopamine neurons , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Time, rate, and conditioning. , 2000, Psychological review.

[11]  Ellen Poliakoff,et al.  Good vibrations: Human interval timing in the vibrotactile modality , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[12]  Wei Xu,et al.  Advances and challenges in log analysis , 2011, Commun. ACM.

[13]  R. Luce,et al.  Evidence from auditory simple reaction times for both change and level detectors , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[14]  Y. Niv,et al.  Temporal Specificity of Reward Prediction Errors Signaled by Putative Dopamine Neurons in Rat VTA Depends on Ventral Striatum , 2016, Neuron.

[15]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Dissociable Reward and Timing Signals in Human Midbrain and Ventral Striatum , 2011, Neuron.

[16]  J. Gibbon Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. , 1977 .

[17]  E. Wagenmakers,et al.  Psychological interpretation of the ex-Gaussian and shifted Wald parameters: A diffusion model analysis , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[18]  Bethany C. Kordella A Diffusion Model Analysis of the Effects of Aging on Sentence Memory , 2009 .

[19]  H. W. Ades,et al.  CORTICAL RESPONSE OF THE ANESTHETIZED CAT TO GROSS PHOTIC AND ELECTRICAL AFFERENT STIMULATION , 1943 .

[20]  Catalin V. Buhusi,et al.  What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[21]  Eugene Galanter,et al.  Handbook of mathematical psychology: I. , 1963 .

[22]  S. Grondin,et al.  Judging the relative duration of multimodal short empty time intervals , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[23]  A. Nobre,et al.  Endogenous modulation of low frequency oscillations by temporal expectations , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  Charles S. Myers,et al.  On reaction times. , 1909 .

[25]  P. Montague,et al.  Ready…Go: Amplitude of the fMRI Signal Encodes Expectation of Cue Arrival Time , 2009, PLoS biology.

[26]  R Parasuraman,et al.  Brain events underlying detection and recognition of weak sensory signals. , 1980, Science.

[27]  Dean V. Buonomano,et al.  The Neural Basis of Timing: Distributed Mechanisms for Diverse Functions , 2018, Neuron.

[28]  B. Repp,et al.  Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of recent research (2006–2012) , 2013, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[29]  Hugo Merchant,et al.  Subsecond timing in primates: comparison of interval production between human subjects and rhesus monkeys. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  M. A. MacIver,et al.  Neuroscience Needs Behavior: Correcting a Reductionist Bias , 2017, Neuron.

[31]  Andrew J. Stewart,et al.  Modality differences in timing and the filled-duration illusion: Testing the pacemaker rate explanation , 2018, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[32]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization , 1986 .

[33]  Geoffrey M. Ghose,et al.  Attentional modulation in visual cortex depends on task timing , 2002, Nature.

[34]  Philippe Lefèvre,et al.  How Do Primates Anticipate Uncertain Future Events? , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[36]  Judith Haldemann,et al.  Auditory and visual temporal sensitivity: evidence for a hierarchical structure of modality-specific and modality-independent levels of temporal information processing , 2011, Psychological Research.

[37]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Toward a neurobiology of temporal cognition: advances and challenges , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[38]  R. Heath,et al.  Sensory Evoked Responses Recorded Simultaneously from Human Cortex and Scalp , 1966, Nature.

[39]  Anna C. Nobre,et al.  Anticipated moments: temporal structure in attention , 2017, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[40]  R H S Carpenter,et al.  The time course of stimulus expectation in a saccadic decision task. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  R. Hohle INFERRED COMPONENTS OF REACTION TIMES AS FUNCTIONS OF FOREPERIOD DURATION. , 1965, Journal of experimental psychology.

[42]  Robert Rousseau,et al.  Duration discrimination of empty time intervals marked by intermodal pulses , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[43]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Scalar expectancy theory and peak-interval timing in humans. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[44]  Geraint Rees,et al.  Encoding of Temporal Probabilities in the Human Brain , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  K. Linkenkaer-Hansen,et al.  Early Neural Correlates of Conscious Somatosensory Perception , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  H. Spekreijse,et al.  Two distinct modes of sensory processing observed in monkey primary visual cortex (V1) , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[47]  György Buzsáki,et al.  Space and time in the brain , 2017, Science.

[48]  E. H. Kemp,et al.  ELECTRIC RESPONSES OF THE BRAIN STEM TO BILATERAL AUDITORY STIMULATION , 1937 .

[49]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Cortical sources of the early components of the visual evoked potential , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[50]  Ralph W. Danielson,et al.  Ocular dominance demonstrated by unconscious sighting , 1929 .

[51]  Andreas Lochbihler,et al.  A tutorial introduction to CryptHOL , 2018, IACR Cryptol. ePrint Arch..