Experimentally confirmed mathematical model for human control of a non-rigid object.

Determining the principles used to plan and execute movements is a fundamental question in neuroscience research. When humans reach to a target with their hand, they exhibit stereotypical movements that closely follow an optimally smooth trajectory. Even when faced with various perceptual or mechanical perturbations, subjects readily adapt their motor output to preserve this stereotypical trajectory. When humans manipulate non-rigid objects, however, they must control the movements of the object as well as the hand. Such tasks impose a fundamentally different control problem than that of moving one's arm alone. Here, we developed a mathematical model for transporting a mass-on-a-spring to a target in an optimally smooth way. We demonstrate that the well-known "minimum-jerk" model for smooth reaching movements cannot accomplish this task. Our model extends the concept of smoothness to allow for the control of non-rigid objects. Although our model makes some predictions that are similar to minimum jerk, it predicts distinctly different optimal trajectories in several specific cases. In particular, when the relative speed of the movement becomes fast enough or when the object stiffness becomes small enough, the model predicts that subjects will transition from a uni-phasic hand motion to a bi-phasic hand motion. We directly tested these predictions in human subjects. Our subjects adopted trajectories that were well-predicted by our model, including all of the predicted transitions between uni- and bi-phasic hand motions. These findings suggest that smoothness of motion is a general principle of movement planning that extends beyond the control of hand trajectories.

[1]  J. Gillis,et al.  Classical dynamics of particles and systems , 1965 .

[2]  J. T. Massey,et al.  Spatial trajectories and reaction times of aimed movements: effects of practice, uncertainty, and change in target location. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  D. B. Preston Spectral Analysis and Time Series , 1983 .

[4]  N. Hogan An organizing principle for a class of voluntary movements , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[5]  T. Flash,et al.  The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[6]  R. H. Myers Classical and modern regression with applications , 1986 .

[7]  R. Miall,et al.  Manual tracking of visual targets by trained monkeys , 1986, Behavioural Brain Research.

[8]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Controlling multijoint motor behavior. , 1987, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[9]  宇野 洋二,et al.  Formation and control of optimal trajectory in human multijoint arm movement : minimum torque-change model , 1988 .

[10]  O. Bock Load compensation in human goal-directed arm movements , 1990, Behavioural Brain Research.

[11]  M. A. Arbib,et al.  A Model of the Effects of Speed, Accuracy, and Perturbation on Visually Guided Reaching , 1992 .

[12]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Early stages of load compensation in human aimed arm movements , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.

[13]  Stefan Schaal,et al.  Open loop stable control strategies for robot juggling , 1993, [1993] Proceedings IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Automation.

[14]  M. Kawato,et al.  Inverse-dynamics model eye movement control by Purkinje cells in the cerebellum , 1993, Nature.

[15]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Is the cerebellum a smith predictor? , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.

[16]  Heekuck Oh,et al.  Neural Networks for Pattern Recognition , 1993, Adv. Comput..

[17]  S. Hah,et al.  The relative dominance of schemata in a manual tracking task: input patterns, system dynamics, and movement patterns. , 1994, Journal of motor behavior.

[18]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[19]  J. Lackner,et al.  Rapid adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of arm trajectory. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[21]  J. Lackner,et al.  Motor adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of reaching movements: endpoint but not trajectory adaptation transfers to the nonexposed arm. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control , 1996, Neural Networks.

[23]  S. Schaal,et al.  One-Handed Juggling: A Dynamical Approach to a Rhythmic Movement Task. , 1996, Journal of motor behavior.

[24]  R. Miall,et al.  Task-dependent changes in visual feedback control: a frequency analysis of human manual tracking. , 1996, Journal of motor behavior.

[25]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Equilibrium-Point Control Hypothesis Examined by Measured Arm Stiffness During Multijoint Movement , 1996, Science.

[26]  E. Brenner,et al.  Fast Responses of the Human Hand to Changes in Target Position. , 1997, Journal of motor behavior.

[27]  R. McN. Alexander,et al.  A minimum energy cost hypothesis for human arm trajectories , 1997, Biological Cybernetics.

[28]  Alexander Rm,et al.  A minimum energy cost hypothesis for human arm trajectories. , 1997 .

[29]  H. J. Wyatt,et al.  Offset dynamics of human smooth pursuit eye movements: Effects of target presence and subject attention , 1997, Vision Research.

[30]  W.Z. Rymer,et al.  Role of intrinsic muscle properties in producing smooth movements , 1997, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[31]  J R Flanagan,et al.  The Role of Internal Models in Motion Planning and Control: Evidence from Grip Force Adjustments during Movements of Hand-Held Loads , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  Gideon F. Inbar,et al.  A model for learning human reaching movements , 1996, Biological Cybernetics.

[33]  W. T. Thach A Role for the Cerebellum in Learning Movement Coordination , 1998, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[34]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Making smooth moves , 2022 .

[35]  T. Ebner A role for the cerebellum in the control of limb movement velocity , 1998, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[36]  M. Kawato,et al.  Temporal firing patterns of Purkinje cells in the cerebellar ventral paraflocculus during ocular following responses in monkeys I. Simple spikes. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[38]  Kevin M. Lynch,et al.  Dynamic Nonprehensile Manipulation: Controllability, Planning, and Experiments , 1999, Int. J. Robotics Res..

[39]  J R Flanagan,et al.  Composition and Decomposition of Internal Models in Motor Learning under Altered Kinematic and Dynamic Environments , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  H. Eng,et al.  Synthesis of β-Tubulin, Actin, and Other Proteins in Axons of Sympathetic Neurons in Compartmented Cultures , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  Y Uno,et al.  Quantitative examinations of internal representations for arm trajectory planning: minimum commanded torque change model. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Spatio-Temporal Prediction Modulates the Perception of Self-Produced Stimuli , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[43]  W. McKeon Sources of health care data for social work leaders. , 1999, Continuum.

[44]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Computational nature of human adaptive control during learning of reaching movements in force fields , 1999, Biological Cybernetics.

[45]  John W. Krakauer,et al.  Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[46]  D J Ostry,et al.  Compensation for interaction torques during single- and multijoint limb movement. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  Denis Mottet,et al.  The regulation of release parameters in underarm precision throwing , 2000, Journal of sports sciences.

[48]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Computational principles of movement neuroscience , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[49]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Learning of action through adaptive combination of motor primitives , 2000, Nature.

[50]  K. Doya Complementary roles of basal ganglia and cerebellum in learning and motor control , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[51]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[52]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Why can't you tickle yourself? , 2000, Neuroreport.

[53]  D. Wolpert,et al.  The cerebellum is involved in predicting the sensory consequences of action , 1999, Neuroreport.

[54]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Does the brain model Newton's laws? , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[55]  J. Flanagan,et al.  The Inertial Anisotropy of the Arm Is Accurately Predicted during Movement Planning , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[56]  Sascha E. Engelbrecht,et al.  Minimum Principles in Motor Control. , 2001, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[57]  Tamar Flash,et al.  Computational approaches to motor control , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[58]  D Sternad,et al.  Dynamics of a bouncing ball in human performance. , 2000, Physical review. E, Statistical, nonlinear, and soft matter physics.

[59]  R. Brent Gillespie,et al.  Haptic feedback and human performance in a dynamic task , 2002, Proceedings 10th Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems. HAPTICS 2002.

[60]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[61]  Christopher D Mah,et al.  Manipulating objects with internal degrees of freedom: evidence for model-based control. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[62]  Stefan Schaal,et al.  Forward models in visuomotor control. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[63]  Stephen H. Scott,et al.  Overlap of internal models in motor cortex for mechanical loads during reaching , 2002, Nature.

[64]  Ferdinando A. Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Evidence for a specific internal representation of motion–force relationships during object manipulation , 2003, Biological Cybernetics.

[65]  R. Johansson,et al.  Prediction Precedes Control in Motor Learning , 2003, Current Biology.

[66]  Christopher D Mah,et al.  Generalization of Object Manipulation Skills Learned without Limb Motion , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[67]  M. Kawato,et al.  Formation and control of optimal trajectory in human multijoint arm movement , 1989, Biological Cybernetics.

[68]  W. L. Nelson Physical principles for economies of skilled movements , 1983, Biological Cybernetics.

[69]  A. Bahill,et al.  Determining ideal baseball bat weights using muscle force-velocity relationships , 1989, Biological Cybernetics.

[70]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Are arm trajectories planned in kinematic or dynamic coordinates? An adaptation study , 1995, Experimental Brain Research.