‘Ghost’ experiments and the dissection of social learning in humans and animals

The focus of this review is the experimental techniques used to identify forms of social learning shown by humans and nonhuman animals. Specifically, the ‘ghost display’ and ‘end‐state’ conditions, which have been used to tease apart imitative and emulative learning are evaluated. In a ghost display, the movements of an apparatus are demonstrated, often through the discrete use of fishing‐line or hidden mechanisms, without a live model acting directly upon the apparatus so that the apparatus appears to be operated as if by a ‘ghostly’ agent. In an end‐state condition, an observing individual is shown the initial state of the test apparatus, the apparatus is then manipulated out‐of‐sight and then represented to the individual in its final state. The aim of the ghost display condition is to determine whether individuals are able to emulate by replicating the movements of an apparatus, or perform a task, without requiring information about the bodily movements required to do so (imitation). The end‐state condition is used to identify goal‐emulation by assessing whether the observer can replicate the steps required to solve the task without having been shown the required body actions or task movements. The responses of individuals tested with either the ghost display and/or end‐state conditions are compared to those of further individuals who have observed a full demonstration by either a human experimenter or a conspecific. The responses of a control group, to whom no information has been provided about the test apparatus or required actions, are also compared and evaluated. The efficacy of these experimental techniques employed with humans, nonhuman primates, dogs, rats and birds are discussed and evaluated. The experiments reviewed herein emphasise the need to provide ghost displays and end‐state conditions in combination, along with full live demonstrations and a no‐information control. Future research directions are proposed.

[1]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Re-enacting intended acts: Comparing 12- and 18-month-olds , 1999 .

[2]  Holly C. Miller,et al.  Imitation and emulation by dogs using a bidirectional control procedure , 2009, Behavioural Processes.

[3]  Andrew Whiten,et al.  Social Learning of an Artificial Fruit Task in Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus apella) , 1999 .

[4]  C. Lumsden Culture and the Evolutionary Process, Robert Boyd, Peter J. Richerson. University of Chicago Press, Chicago & London (1985), viii, +301. Price $29.95 , 1986 .

[5]  A. Whiten,et al.  Cultural Transmission of Tool Use in Young Children: A Diffusion Chain Study , 2008 .

[6]  C. Tolman,et al.  Social facilitation of feeding behaviour in the domestic chick , 1964 .

[7]  J. Leca,et al.  Japanese macaque cultures: Inter- and intra-troop behavioural variability of stone handling patterns across 10 troops , 2007 .

[8]  K. Laland Social learning strategies , 2004, Learning & behavior.

[9]  Jennifer E. Sutton,et al.  True Imitative Learning in Pigeons , 1996 .

[10]  G. R. Dawson,et al.  Imitation in rats (Rattus norvegicus): The role of demonstrator action , 1994, Behavioural Processes.

[11]  H. Terrace,et al.  Cognitive Imitation in Rhesus Macaques , 2004, Science.

[12]  Stephen C. Want,et al.  How do children ape? Applying concepts from the study of non-human primates to the developmental study of 'imitation' in children , 2002 .

[13]  R. Zajonc SOCIAL FACILITATION. , 1965, Science.

[14]  C. Heyes,et al.  Limitations of a Bidirectional Control Procedure for the Investigation of Imitation in Rats: Odour Cues on the Manipulandum , 1999 .

[15]  S. A. Barnett,et al.  Learning and instinct in animals. , 1957 .

[16]  József Topál,et al.  Infants' Perseverative Search Errors Are Induced by Pragmatic Misinterpretation , 2008, Science.

[17]  12-Month-Olds Produce Others' Intended but Unfulfilled Acts. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[18]  Lydia M. Hopper,et al.  Experimental studies of traditions and underlying transmission processes in chimpanzees , 2007, Animal Behaviour.

[19]  T. Charman,et al.  Delineating the role of stimulus enhancement and emulation learning in the behavioural re-enactment paradigm , 2002 .

[20]  R. Byrne Imitation of novel complex actions: What does the evidence from animals mean? , 2002 .

[21]  M. Tomasello Cultural transmission in the tool use and communicatory signaling of chimpanzees , 1990 .

[22]  Michelle Y. Merrill,et al.  Orangutan Cultures and the Evolution of Material Culture , 2003, Science.

[23]  Á. Miklósi,et al.  How does dominance rank status affect individual and social learning performance in the dog (Canis familiaris)? , 2007, Animal Cognition.

[24]  Imitation as a learning mechanism and research tool: how does imitation interact with other cognitive functions? , 2002 .

[25]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Copying results and copying actions in the process of social learning: chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens) , 2005, Animal Cognition.

[26]  Magnus Enquist,et al.  CRITICAL POINTS IN CURRENT THEORY OF CONFORMIST SOCIAL LEARNING , 2007 .

[27]  J. Stevenson The cultural origins of human cognition , 2001 .

[28]  Stephen C. Want,et al.  Learning from other people's mistakes: causal understanding in learning to use a tool. , 2001, Child development.

[29]  Richard B. D’Eath,et al.  Can video images imitate real stimuli in animal behaviour experiments? , 1998 .

[30]  D. Wood Social interaction as tutoring , 1989 .

[31]  M. Nielsen,et al.  Copying actions and copying outcomes: social learning through the second year. , 2006, Developmental psychology.

[32]  B. Galef,et al.  Imitation learning in budgerigars: Dawson and Foss (1965) revisited , 1986, Behavioural Processes.

[33]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Imitation of Facial and Manual Gestures by Human Neonates , 1977, Science.

[34]  Michelle Press,et al.  Manipulative monkeys: The capuchins of lomas barbudal. , 2009 .

[35]  Lydia M. Hopper,et al.  Observational learning of tool use in children: Investigating cultural spread through diffusion chains and learning mechanisms through ghost displays. , 2010, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[36]  Betty V. Dawson,et al.  Observational learning in budgerigars. , 1965, Animal behaviour.

[37]  Chrystopher L. Nehaniv,et al.  Three Sources of Information in Social Learning , 2002 .

[38]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Understanding "prior intentions" enables two-year-olds to imitatively learn a complex task. , 2002, Child development.

[39]  R. Byrne Emulation in apes: verdict ‘not proven’ , 2002 .

[40]  Thomas R. Zentall,et al.  Can Imitation in Pigeons be Explained by Local Enhancement Together with Trial-and-Error Learning? , 1997 .

[41]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[42]  C. Nicol The social transmission of information and behaviour , 1995 .

[43]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Processes of social learning in the tool use of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). , 1993, Journal of comparative psychology.

[44]  C. Heyes Imitation, culture and cognition , 1993, Animal Behaviour.

[45]  Alex Mesoudi,et al.  The Hierarchical Transformation of Event Knowledge in Human Cultural Transmission , 2004 .

[46]  R. Byrne,et al.  Priming primates: Human and otherwise , 1998, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[47]  B. Galef The question of animal culture , 1992, Human nature.

[48]  Mark Nielsen The social motivation for social learning , 2008 .

[49]  Tony Charman,et al.  Gradations of emulation learning in infants' imitation of actions on objects. , 2005, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[50]  Tetsuro Matsuzawa,et al.  Cognitive Development in Chimpanzees , 2021, Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology.

[51]  Andrew Whiten,et al.  The evolution of animal ‘cultures’ and social intelligence , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[52]  A. Whiten,et al.  Cultures in chimpanzees , 1999, Nature.

[53]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Fourteen-through 18-month-old infants di eren-tially imitate intentional and accidental actions , 1998 .

[54]  P. Richerson,et al.  Why Culture is Common, but Cultural Evolution is Rare , 1996 .

[55]  G. Goldenberg,et al.  Tool use and mechanical problem solving in apraxia , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[56]  I. Uzgiris,et al.  Interpersonal interaction as foundation for cultural learning , 1993, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[57]  David A. Leavens,et al.  Theory of mind in nonhuman primates , 1998, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[58]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Push or Pull: Imitation vs. Emulation in Great Apes and Human Children , 2006 .

[59]  A. Whiten,et al.  CHARTING CULTURAL VARIATION IN CHIMPANZEES , 2001 .

[60]  I. Užgiris Two Functions of Imitation During Infancy , 1981 .

[61]  M. Carpenter,et al.  On imitation in apes and children , 2003 .

[62]  L. Huber,et al.  Selective Imitation in Domestic Dogs , 2007, Current Biology.

[63]  B. Galef,et al.  Effects of environmental stability and demonstrator age on social learning of food preferences by young Norway rats , 2004, Animal Behaviour.

[64]  Alexandra Horowitz Do humans ape? Or do apes human? Imitation and intention in humans (Homo sapiens) and other animals. , 2003, Journal of comparative psychology.

[65]  T R Zentall,et al.  Imitative learning in male Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) using the two-action method. , 1996, Journal of comparative psychology.

[66]  J. Deloache,et al.  The medium can obscure the message: young children's understanding of video. , 1998, Child development.

[67]  Andrew Whiten,et al.  Imitation of causally opaque versus causally transparent tool use by 3- and 5-year-old children , 2007 .

[68]  A. Meltzoff Immediate and Deferred Imitation in Fourteen- and Twenty-Four-Month-Old Infants. , 1985 .

[69]  A. Whiten,et al.  On the Nature and Evolution of Imitation in the Animal Kingdom: Reappraisal of a Century of Research , 1992 .

[70]  Thomas Suddendorf,et al.  Visual self-recognition in mirrors and live videos: Evidence for a developmental asynchrony , 2007 .

[71]  C. Heyes,et al.  Infants' behavioral reenactment of "failed attempts": exploring the roles of emulation learning, stimulus enhancement, and understanding of intentions. , 2002, Developmental psychology.

[72]  A. Meltzoff Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.

[73]  P. H. Duerfeldt,et al.  Some relevant variables in observational learning of the rat , 1971 .

[74]  A. Whiten,et al.  The multiple roles of cultural transmission experiments in understanding human cultural evolution , 2008, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[75]  A. Whiten,et al.  Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens) , 2005, Animal Cognition.

[76]  J. Russell,et al.  The ghost condition: imitation versus emulation in young children's observational learning. , 2004, Developmental psychology.

[77]  Change in children's understanding of others' intentional actions. , 2006, Developmental science.

[78]  Luc-Alain Giraldeau,et al.  Vicarious sampling: the use of personal and public information by starlings foraging in a simple patchy environment , 1996, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[79]  Andrew Whiten,et al.  Imitation of hierarchical structure versus component details of complex actions by 3- and 5-year-olds. , 2008, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[80]  T. Bugnyar,et al.  Push or pull: an experimental study on imitation in marmosets , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[81]  Thomas R. Zentall,et al.  IMITATION IN ANIMALS: EVIDENCE, FUNCTION, AND MECHANISMS , 2001, Cybern. Syst..

[82]  W. Prinz,et al.  Goal representations in imitative actions , 2002 .

[83]  C. V. van Schaik,et al.  A model for tool-use traditions in primates: implications for the coevolution of culture and cognition. , 2003, Journal of human evolution.

[84]  Gabrielle A. Strouse,et al.  "Don't try this at home": toddlers' imitation of new skills from people on video. , 2008, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[85]  D. Maestripieri,et al.  Mother-infant interactions in western Lowland Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla): spatial relationships, communication, and opportunities for social learning. , 2002, Journal of comparative psychology.

[86]  A. Whiten,et al.  Scrounging facilitates social learning in common marmosets, Callithrix jacchus , 2003, Animal Behaviour.

[87]  Imitation is mediated by many goals, not just one [Letter] , 2002 .

[88]  Thomas R Zentall,et al.  Imitation and affordance learning by pigeons (Columba livia). , 2003, Journal of comparative psychology.

[89]  G. Simcock,et al.  The effect of social engagement on 24-month-olds' imitation from live and televised models. , 2008, Developmental science.

[90]  Thomas R. Zentall,et al.  Imitation: definitions, evidence, and mechanisms , 2006, Animal Cognition.

[91]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Children's coding of human action: cognitive factors influencing imitation in 3-year-olds. , 2000, Developmental science.

[92]  John A. Corson,et al.  Observational learning of a lever pressing response , 1967 .

[93]  Herbert A. Lurie,et al.  Cognitive imitation in typically-developing 3- and 4-year olds and individuals with autism , 2007 .

[94]  K. Laland,et al.  Response facilitation in the domestic fowl , 2007, Animal Behaviour.

[95]  C. Heyes,et al.  A Demonstration of Observational Learning in Rats using a Bidirectional Control , 1990, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[96]  A. Whiten,et al.  How do apes ape? , 2004, Learning & behavior.

[97]  Lydia M. Hopper,et al.  Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[98]  T. W. Fawcett,et al.  A test of imitative learning in starlings using a two-action method with an enhanced ghost control , 2002, Animal Behaviour.

[99]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Chimpanzee and Human Cultures , 1998, Current Anthropology.

[100]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Action generation and action perception in imitation: an instance of the ideomotor principle. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[101]  Patricia J. Bauer,et al.  To ‘ape’ or to emulate? Young children’s use of both strategies in a single study , 2002 .

[102]  M. Bitterman THE EVOLUTION OF INTELLIGENCE. , 1965, Scientific American.

[103]  I. Užgiris The Social Context of Infant Imitation , 1991 .

[104]  B. Voelkl,et al.  True imitation in marmosets , 2000, Animal Behaviour.

[105]  Emily D. Klein,et al.  Imitative learning in Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) using the bidirectional control procedure , 2002, Animal learning & behavior.

[106]  Lydia M Hopper,et al.  Observational learning in chimpanzees and children studied through ‘ghost’ conditions , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[107]  A. Whiten,et al.  Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees , 2005, Nature.

[108]  A. Whiten,et al.  A potent effect of observational learning on chimpanzee tool construction , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[109]  D. Biro,et al.  Cultural innovation and transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees: evidence from field experiments , 2003, Animal Cognition.

[110]  J. Nadel,et al.  The evolving nature of imitation as a format for communication. , 1999 .

[111]  T Matsuzawa,et al.  Imitation of intentional manipulatory actions in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). , 2000, Journal of comparative psychology.

[112]  György Gergely,et al.  The role of behavioral cues in understanding goal-directed actions in infancy. , 2007, Progress in brain research.

[113]  Justin N Wood,et al.  The Perception of Rational, Goal-Directed Action in Nonhuman Primates , 2007, Science.

[114]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Observational learning of tool-use by young chimpanzees , 1987 .

[115]  Robert Wall,et al.  Push or pull , 2008 .

[116]  M. Carpenter Instrumental, social, and shared goals and intentions in imitation , 2006 .

[117]  Daniel R. Anderson,et al.  Television and Very Young Children , 2005 .

[118]  C. Heyes,et al.  Preschoolers' Behavioural Reenactment of ''Failed Attempts'': The Roles of Intention-Reading, Emulation and Mimicry. , 2006 .

[119]  Lydia M. Hopper Dissection of observational learning among chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens) , 2008 .

[120]  I. Coolen,et al.  Trade‐Offs in the Adaptive Use of Social and Asocial Learning , 2005 .

[121]  J. Leca,et al.  Food provisioning and stone handling tradition in Japanese macaques: a comparative study of ten troops , 2008, American journal of primatology.