Distinct recruitment of temporo-parietal junction and medial prefrontal cortex in behavior understanding and trait identification

It has been suggested that the temporo-parietal junction (TPJ) is involved in inferring immediate goals and intentions from behaviors, whereas the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) integrates social information, such as traits, at a more abstract level. To explore the differential role of the TPJ and mPFC, participants read several verbal descriptions about an agent. Embedded in a factorial design, in one-half of the trials (behavior condition), the agent was engaged in a simple goal-directed behavior, whereas in the other half this description was absent. In another half of the trials (trait condition), the participants had to answer a question about a trait of the agent, whereas in the other half the question was about the agent's physical appearance. The results revealed that the dorsal mPFC was recruited when participants inferred the agent's trait, irrespective of a behavioral description. In contrast, the TPJ, posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS), anterior intraparietal sulcus, and premotor cortex were activated when goal-directed behavioral information was presented, irrespective of a trait question. These findings confirm that in a social context, the TPJ (and pSTS) is activated for understanding goal-directed behaviors, whereas the mPFC is involved in processing traits.

[1]  S. Huettel,et al.  Contributions of frontopolar cortex to judgments about self, others and relations. , 2011, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[2]  Ruth Seurinck,et al.  Spontaneous and intentional trait inferences recruit a common mentalizing network to a different degree: Spontaneous inferences activate only its core areas , 2011, Social neuroscience.

[3]  F. Overwalle A dissociation between social mentalizing and general reasoning , 2011, NeuroImage.

[4]  J. Hodgins,et al.  Exploring the neural correlates of goal-directed action and intention understanding , 2011, NeuroImage.

[5]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Dissociable Neural Systems Support Retrieval of How and Why Action Knowledge , 2010, Psychological science.

[6]  R. Saxe,et al.  What gets the attention of the temporo-parietal junction? An fMRI investigation of attention and theory of mind , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  Gregory McCarthy,et al.  Reading about the actions of others: Biological motion imagery and action congruency influence brain activity , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  William W. Graves,et al.  Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  F. Overwalle,et al.  Understanding others' actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: A meta-analysis , 2009, NeuroImage.

[10]  M. Vandekerckhove,et al.  Trait inferences in goal-directed behavior: ERP timing and localization under spontaneous and intentional processing. , 2009, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[11]  Josef Perner,et al.  Temporo-parietal Junction Activity in Theory-of-Mind Tasks: Falseness, Beliefs, or Attention , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  Emery N. Brown,et al.  Distinct Regions of Right Temporo-Parietal Junction Are Selective for Theory of Mind and Exogenous Attention , 2009, PloS one.

[13]  F. Van Overwalle,et al.  Social cognition and the brain: A meta‐analysis , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[14]  Jan Gläscher,et al.  Visualization of Group Inference Data in Functional Neuroimaging , 2009, Neuroinformatics.

[15]  M. Corbetta,et al.  The Reorienting System of the Human Brain: From Environment to Theory of Mind , 2008, Neuron.

[16]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Action outcomes are represented in human inferior frontoparietal cortex. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror system and its role in social cognition , 2008, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[18]  Jason P. Mitchell Activity in right temporo-parietal junction is not selective for theory-of-mind. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[19]  Karla K. Evans,et al.  Spontaneous retrieval of affective person knowledge in face perception , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  H. Walter,et al.  The intentional network: How the brain reads varieties of intentions , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  J. Decety,et al.  The Role of the Right Temporoparietal Junction in Social Interaction: How Low-Level Computational Processes Contribute to Meta-Cognition , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[22]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Event frequency modulates the processing of daily life activities in human medial prefrontal cortex. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[23]  F. Van Overwalle,et al.  Electrophysiological time course and brain areas of spontaneous and intentional trait inferences. , 2007, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[24]  C. Frith,et al.  Adolescent development of the neural circuitry for thinking about intentions , 2007, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[25]  E. Denkova,et al.  Neural correlates of remembering/knowing famous people: An event-related fMRI study , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  M. Kronbichler,et al.  Thinking of mental and other representations: The roles of left and right temporo-parietal junction , 2006, Social neuroscience.

[27]  William M. Kelley,et al.  Neuroanatomical Evidence for Distinct Cognitive and Affective Components of Self , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[28]  G. Glover,et al.  Cultural and linguistic influence on neural bases of ‘Theory of Mind’: An fMRI study with Japanese bilinguals , 2006, Brain and Language.

[29]  Bryan T. Denny,et al.  Medial prefrontal activity differentiates self from close others. , 2006, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[30]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Medial prefrontal dissociations during processing of trait diagnostic and nondiagnostic person information. , 2006, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[31]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Dissociable Medial Prefrontal Contributions to Judgments of Similar and Dissimilar Others , 2006, Neuron.

[32]  W. Schneider,et al.  Perceptual Knowledge Retrieval Activates Sensory Brain Regions , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  R. Saxe Uniquely human social cognition , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[34]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Goal Representation in Human Anterior Intraparietal Sulcus , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Differential role of the orbital frontal lobe in emotional versus cognitive perspective-taking , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  R. Saxe,et al.  Making sense of another mind: The role of the right temporo-parietal junction , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  Alexander Todorov,et al.  Attributions on the brain: Neuro-imaging dispositional inferences, beyond theory of mind , 2005, NeuroImage.

[38]  Michael S. Beauchamp,et al.  Automatic Priming of Semantically Related Words Reduces Activity in the Fusiform Gyrus , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  C. Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2005, Current Biology.

[40]  C. Neil Macrae,et al.  The Link between Social Cognition and Self-referential Thought in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  Evelyn C. Ferstl,et al.  Emotional and Temporal Aspects of Situation Model Processing during Text Comprehension: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[43]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[44]  Mahzarin R. Banaji,et al.  Encoding-Specific Effects of Social Cognition on the Neural Correlates of Subsequent Memory , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  D. H Weissman,et al.  Conflict monitoring in the human anterior cingulate cortex during selective attention to global and local object features , 2003, NeuroImage.

[46]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[47]  J. Kounios,et al.  Category-specific medial temporal lobe activation and the consolidation of semantic memory: evidence from fMRI. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[48]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[49]  J. Hiltunen,et al.  Different brain activation patterns during production of animals versus artefacts: a PET activation study on category-specific processing. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[50]  J. Gee,et al.  Neural representation of verb meaning: An fMRI study , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[51]  K. Zilles,et al.  Mind Reading: Neural Mechanisms of Theory of Mind and Self-Perspective , 2001, NeuroImage.

[52]  T. Allison,et al.  Word recognition in the human inferior temporal lobe , 1994, Nature.

[53]  L. Johnston It's the thought that counts-- , 1994 .

[54]  R. Mar The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. , 2011, Annual review of psychology.

[55]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Identifying the What, Why, and How of an Observed Action: An fMRI Study of Mentalizing and Mechanizing during Action Observation , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[56]  V. Yzerbyt,et al.  Social judgeability : the impact of meta-informational cues on the use of stereotypes , 1994 .

[57]  Frank Van Overwalle,et al.  Please Scroll down for Article Social Neuroscience Erp Time Course and Brain Areas of Spontaneous and Intentional Goal Inferences Erp Time Course and Brain Areas of Spontaneous and Intentional Goal Inferences , 2022 .