The Common Neural Basis of Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, Navigation, Theory of Mind, and the Default Mode: A Quantitative Meta-analysis

A core brain network has been proposed to underlie a number of different processes, including remembering, prospection, navigation, and theory of mind [Buckner, R. L., & Carroll, D. C. Self-projection and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 49–57, 2007]. This purported network—medial prefrontal, medial-temporal, and medial and lateral parietal regions—is similar to that observed during default-mode processing and has been argued to represent self-projection [Buckner, R. L., & Carroll, D. C. Self-projection and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 49–57, 2007] or scene-construction [Hassabis, D., & Maguire, E. A. Deconstructing episodic memory with construction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 299–306, 2007]. To date, no systematic and quantitative demonstration of evidence for this common network has been presented. Using the activation likelihood estimation (ALE) approach, we conducted four separate quantitative meta-analyses of neuroimaging studies on: (a) autobiographical memory, (b) navigation, (c) theory of mind, and (d) default mode. A conjunction analysis between these domains demonstrated a high degree of correspondence. We compared these findings to a separate ALE analysis of prospection studies and found additional correspondence. Across all domains, and consistent with the proposed network, correspondence was found within the medial-temporal lobe, precuneus, posterior cingulate, retrosplenial cortex, and the temporo-parietal junction. Additionally, this study revealed that the core network extends to lateral prefrontal and occipital cortices. Autobiographical memory, prospection, theory of mind, and default mode demonstrated further reliable involvement of the medial prefrontal cortex and lateral temporal cortices. Autobiographical memory and theory of mind, previously studied as distinct, exhibited extensive functional overlap. These findings represent quantitative evidence for a core network underlying a variety of cognitive domains.

[1]  E. Tulving,et al.  Toward a theory of episodic memory: the frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[2]  Donald T. Stuss,et al.  Common and Unique Neural Correlates of Autobiographical Memory and Theory of Mind , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  Dharshan Kumaran,et al.  The Human Hippocampus: Cognitive Maps or Relational Memory? , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Wandering Minds: The Default Network and Stimulus-Independent Thought , 2007, Science.

[5]  Joan Peskin,et al.  The effects of adding metacognitive language to story texts , 2004 .

[6]  R. Mar The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. , 2011, Annual review of psychology.

[7]  Olaf Blanke,et al.  Subjective mental time: the functional architecture of projecting the self to past and future , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[8]  Michael D. Greicius,et al.  Development of functional and structural connectivity within the default mode network in young children , 2010, NeuroImage.

[9]  M. Kronbichler,et al.  Thinking of mental and other representations: The roles of left and right temporo-parietal junction , 2006, Social neuroscience.

[10]  E. Maguire,et al.  The Well-Worn Route and the Path Less Traveled Distinct Neural Bases of Route Following and Wayfinding in Humans , 2003, Neuron.

[11]  Kevin S. LaBar,et al.  Co-activation of the amygdala, hippocampus and inferior frontal gyrus during autobiographical memory retrieval , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  H. Eichenbaum A cortical–hippocampal system for declarative memory , 2000, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[13]  Michael B. Miller,et al.  Neural Correlates of Detecting Pretense: Automatic Engagement of the Intentional Stance under Covert Conditions , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  Hongkeun Kim,et al.  Dissociating the roles of the default-mode, dorsal, and ventral networks in episodic memory retrieval , 2010, NeuroImage.

[15]  Cheryl L. Grady,et al.  The default network and processing of personally relevant information: Converging evidence from task-related modulations and functional connectivity , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  M. Denis,et al.  Neural basis of mental scanning of a topographic representation built from a text. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  James K. Nelson,et al.  Age Differences in Deactivation: A Link to Cognitive Control? , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  V. Menon,et al.  Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function , 2010, Brain Structure and Function.

[19]  Ingrid R. Olson,et al.  Similarities and differences between parietal and frontal patients in autobiographical and constructed experience tasks , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  Franco Cauda,et al.  Different functions in the cingulate cortex, a meta-analytic connectivity modeling study , 2011, NeuroImage.

[21]  Alex Martin,et al.  Semantic memory and the brain: structure and processes , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[22]  Hans-Jochen Heinze,et al.  Different cortical activations for subjects using allocentric or egocentric strategies in a virtual navigation task , 2004, Neuroreport.

[23]  Philip McGuire,et al.  Brain activity during stimulus independent thought. , 1996 .

[24]  María Ruz,et al.  Emotional conflict in interpersonal interactions , 2011, NeuroImage.

[25]  J. A. Frost,et al.  Conceptual Processing during the Conscious Resting State: A Functional MRI Study , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  Daniel P. Kennedy,et al.  Failing to deactivate: resting functional abnormalities in autism. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[27]  L. Squire,et al.  The medial temporal lobe memory system , 1991, Science.

[28]  E. Maguire,et al.  What does the retrosplenial cortex do? , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[29]  Kevin N. Ochsner,et al.  The neural correlates of direct and reflected self-knowledge , 2005, NeuroImage.

[30]  E A Maguire,et al.  Hippocampal involvement in human topographical memory: evidence from functional imaging. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[31]  Daniela Montaldi,et al.  Recalling spatial information as a component of recently and remotely acquired episodic or semantic memories: an fMRI study. , 2004, Neuropsychology.

[32]  F. Eustache,et al.  Le réseau cérébral par défaut : rôle cognitif et perturbations dans la pathologie , 2010 .

[33]  John A. E. Anderson,et al.  A multivariate analysis of age-related differences in default mode and task-positive networks across multiple cognitive domains. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[34]  M. Greicius,et al.  Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer's disease from healthy aging: Evidence from functional MRI , 2004, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA.

[35]  G. Shulman,et al.  Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[36]  Demis Hassabis,et al.  Imagining fictitious and future experiences: Evidence from developmental amnesia , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  Amanda Miles,et al.  Functional neuroimaging of emotionally intense autobiographical memories in post-traumatic stress disorder. , 2011, Journal of psychiatric research.

[38]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Functional neuroimaging of autobiographical memory , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[39]  Philip L. Jackson,et al.  The study of social cognition with neuroimaging methods as a means to explore future directions of deficit evaluation in schizophrenia? , 2011, Psychiatry Research.

[40]  Brian Levine,et al.  Theory of Mind Is Independent of Episodic Memory , 2007, Science.

[41]  Fenna M. Krienen,et al.  Segregated Fronto-Cerebellar Circuits Revealed by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[42]  Roberto Cabeza,et al.  Gender differences in autobiographical memory for everyday events: Retrieval elicited by SenseCam images versus verbal cues , 2011, Memory.

[43]  Morris Moscovitch,et al.  Recollective qualities modulate hippocampal activation during autobiographical memory retrieval , 2004, Hippocampus.

[44]  Perrine Ruby,et al.  Distinct Regions of the Medial Prefrontal Cortex Are Associated with Self-referential Processing and Perspective Taking , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  Keith Oatley,et al.  Why Fiction May be Twice as True as Fact: Fiction as Cognitive and Emotional Simulation , 1999 .

[46]  D. Schacter,et al.  The Brain's Default Network , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[47]  G. Winocur,et al.  The cognitive neuroscience of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[48]  Eleanor A. Maguire,et al.  Spontaneous mentalizing during an interactive real world task: An fMRI study , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[49]  Sterling C. Johnson,et al.  Neural correlates of self-reflection. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[50]  M. Linden,et al.  Phenomenal characteristics associated with projecting oneself back into the past and forward into the future: Influence of valence and temporal distance , 2004, Consciousness and Cognition.

[51]  M. Corballis,et al.  The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? , 2007, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[52]  Takashi Hanakawa,et al.  The neural basis of social tactics: An fMRI study , 2006, NeuroImage.

[53]  R. Nathan Spreng,et al.  Patterns of Brain Activity Supporting Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, and Theory of Mind, and Their Relationship to the Default Mode Network , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[54]  C. Frith,et al.  Movement and Mind: A Functional Imaging Study of Perception and Interpretation of Complex Intentional Movement Patterns , 2000, NeuroImage.

[55]  Martial Van der Linden,et al.  Self-referential reflective activity and its relationship with rest: a PET study , 2005, NeuroImage.

[56]  R. Todd Constable,et al.  Evidence That Autobiographic Memory Retrieval Does Not Become Independent of the Hippocampus: An fMRI Study Contrasting Very Recent with Remote Events , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[57]  D. Hassabis,et al.  Patients with hippocampal amnesia cannot imagine new experiences , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[58]  Derek G. V. Mitchell,et al.  Caught in the act: The impact of audience on the neural response to morally and socially inappropriate behavior , 2006, NeuroImage.

[59]  D. Schacter,et al.  Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[60]  K. Szpunar Evidence for an implicit influence of memory on future thinking , 2010, Memory & cognition.

[61]  H. J. Spiers,et al.  The neuroscience of remote spatial memory: A tale of two cities , 2007, Neuroscience.

[62]  K. Szpunar,et al.  Neural substrates of envisioning the future , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[63]  Asaf Gilboa,et al.  Autobiographical and episodic memory—one and the same? Evidence from prefrontal activation in neuroimaging studies , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[64]  B. Postle,et al.  Maintenance versus Manipulation of Information Held in Working Memory: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 1999, Brain and Cognition.

[65]  G. Fink,et al.  Cerebral Representation of One’s Own Past: Neural Networks Involved in Autobiographical Memory , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[66]  G. Winocur,et al.  Remembering our past: functional neuroanatomy of recollection of recent and very remote personal events. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[67]  F. Bermpohl,et al.  Cortical midline structures and the self , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[68]  G. Winocur,et al.  “I have often walked down this street before”: fMRI Studies on the hippocampus and other structures during mental navigation of an old environment , 2004, Hippocampus.

[69]  Marcus E Raichle,et al.  Intrinsic brain activity sets the stage for expression of motivated behavior , 2005, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[70]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  Attitudes towards the outgroup are predicted by activity in the precuneus in Arabs and Israelis , 2010, NeuroImage.

[71]  Friedrich G. Woermann,et al.  Bi-Hemispheric Engagement in the Retrieval of Autobiographical Episodes , 2006, Behavioural neurology.

[72]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  Reading minds versus following rules: Dissociating theory of mind and executive control in the brain , 2006, Social neuroscience.

[73]  C. Frith,et al.  Dissociable neural pathways for the perception and recognition of expressive and instrumental gestures , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[74]  A Berthoz,et al.  Parietal and hippocampal contribution to topokinetic and topographic memory. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[75]  Rafael Malach,et al.  Stimulus-free thoughts induce differential activation in the human default network , 2011, NeuroImage.

[76]  Henrik Walter,et al.  Understanding Intentions in Social Interaction: The Role of the Anterior Paracingulate Cortex , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[77]  T. Allison,et al.  Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[78]  J. Decety,et al.  From the perception of action to the understanding of intention , 2001, Nature reviews. Neuroscience.

[79]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Recalling Routes around London: Activation of the Right Hippocampus in Taxi Drivers , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[80]  John D. E. Gabrieli,et al.  Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex involvement in evaluating self-generated information , 2001, NeuroImage.

[81]  Vinod Menon,et al.  Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[82]  C. Scheiber,et al.  Implicit emotion during recollection of past events: A nonverbal fMRI study , 2006, Brain Research.

[83]  Peter Carruthers,et al.  Theories of theories of mind: What is acquired – theory-theory versus simulation-theory , 1996 .

[84]  M. Lindquist,et al.  Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging data: current and future directions. , 2007, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[85]  Neil Roberts,et al.  Recalling spatial information as a component of recently and remotely acquired episodic or semantic memories , 2004 .

[86]  C. Frith,et al.  “Hey John”: Signals Conveying Communicative Intention toward the Self Activate Brain Regions Associated with “Mentalizing,” Regardless of Modality , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[87]  C. Frith,et al.  Performance-related activity in medial rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) during low-demand tasks. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[88]  Keith Oatley,et al.  The Function of Fiction is the Abstraction and Simulation of Social Experience , 2008, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[90]  Josef Perner,et al.  Retro- and prospection for mental time travel: Emergence of episodic remembering and mental rotation in 5- to 8-year old children☆ , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.

[91]  Stephen P. Stich,et al.  Folk Psychology: Simulation or Tacit Theory? , 1992 .

[92]  G. Gallup,et al.  Where am I? The neurological correlates of self and other. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[93]  Demis Hassabis,et al.  Differential engagement of brain regions within a ‘core’ network during scene construction , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[94]  Alana T. Wong,et al.  Age-Related Changes in the Episodic Simulation of Future Events , 2008, Psychological science.

[95]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Analysis of intersubject variability in activation: An application to the incidental episodic retrieval during recognition test , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[96]  Hiroshi Fukuda,et al.  Time-dependent contribution of the hippocampal complex when remembering the past: a PET study , 2002, Neuroreport.

[97]  G. Fink,et al.  Being with virtual others: Neural correlates of social interaction , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[98]  Guinevere F. Eden,et al.  Meta-Analysis of the Functional Neuroanatomy of Single-Word Reading: Method and Validation , 2002, NeuroImage.

[99]  M Hutchinson,et al.  Task-specific deactivation patterns in functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 1999, Magnetic resonance imaging.

[100]  Kathryn M. McMillan,et al.  A comparison of label‐based review and ALE meta‐analysis in the Stroop task , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[101]  Shawn Betts,et al.  Cognitive and Metacognitive Activity in Mathematical Problem Solving: Prefrontal and Parietal Patterns It Also Appears to Play a Key Role in Retrieval of Arithmetic , 2022 .

[102]  Morris Moscovitch,et al.  Remote spatial memory in an amnesic person with extensive bilateral hippocampal lesions , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[103]  Lisa Zunshine,et al.  Why We Read Fiction: Theory of Mind and the Novel , 2006 .

[104]  R. Byrne,et al.  Machiavellian intelligence : social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans , 1990 .

[105]  G. Fink,et al.  Dysfunctions in brain networks supporting empathy: An fMRI study in adults with autism spectrum disorders , 2010, Social neuroscience.

[106]  K. Zilles,et al.  Mind Reading: Neural Mechanisms of Theory of Mind and Self-Perspective , 2001, NeuroImage.

[107]  J. Decety,et al.  What you believe versus what you think they believe: a neuroimaging study of conceptual perspective‐taking , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[108]  R. Mar The neuropsychology of narrative: story comprehension, story production and their interrelation , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[109]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Empathy for Pain Involves the Affective but not Sensory Components of Pain , 2004, Science.

[110]  M. Buonocore,et al.  Remembering familiar people: the posterior cingulate cortex and autobiographical memory retrieval , 2001, Neuroscience.

[111]  R. Buckner,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.11 No.2 Self-projection and the brain , 2022 .

[112]  M. Farah The neural basis of mental imagery , 1989, Trends in Neurosciences.

[113]  C. Frith,et al.  Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’ , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[114]  D. Hassabis,et al.  Using Imagination to Understand the Neural Basis of Episodic Memory , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[115]  Rebecca Elliott,et al.  Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and empathy: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in a nonverbal task , 2006, NeuroImage.

[116]  R. N. Spreng,et al.  The temporal distribution of past and future autobiographical events across the lifespan , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[117]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Dissociable Medial Prefrontal Contributions to Judgments of Similar and Dissimilar Others , 2006, Neuron.

[118]  M J Brammer,et al.  Exploring the social brain in schizophrenia: left prefrontal underactivation during mental state attribution. , 2000, The American journal of psychiatry.

[119]  H. Lanfermann,et al.  Engagement of Lateral and Medial Prefrontal Areas in the Ecphory of Sad and Happy Autobiographical Memories , 2003, Cortex.

[120]  E. Maguire,et al.  Patterns of hippocampal‐cortical interaction dissociate temporal lobe memory subsystems , 2000, Hippocampus.

[121]  D. Schacter,et al.  Episodic Simulation of Future Events , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[122]  Kiyotaka Nemoto,et al.  The neural network for the mirror system and mentalizing in normally developed children: an fMRI study , 2004, Neuroreport.

[123]  Olaf Blanke,et al.  The mental time line: An analogue of the mental number line in the mapping of life events , 2009, Consciousness and Cognition.

[124]  Marleen B. Schippers,et al.  Mapping the information flow from one brain to another during gestural communication , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[125]  Ruben C Gur,et al.  Brain activation during autobiographical relationship episode narratives: A core conflictual relationship theme approach , 2010, Psychotherapy research : journal of the Society for Psychotherapy Research.

[126]  Boris Suchan,et al.  Foreseeing the future: Occurrence probability of imagined future events modulates hippocampal activation , 2009, Hippocampus.

[127]  B. Mazoyer,et al.  Cortical networks for working memory and executive functions sustain the conscious resting state in man , 2001, Brain Research Bulletin.

[128]  Perrine Ruby,et al.  A relation between rest and the self in the brain? , 2003, Brain Research Reviews.

[129]  C. Neil Macrae,et al.  The Link between Social Cognition and Self-referential Thought in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[130]  R. Hinde,et al.  Growing Points in Ethology , 1976 .

[131]  R. Buckner,et al.  Functional-Anatomic Fractionation of the Brain's Default Network , 2010, Neuron.

[132]  Alexander Todorov,et al.  Attributions on the brain: Neuro-imaging dispositional inferences, beyond theory of mind , 2005, NeuroImage.

[133]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension , 1995, Cognition.

[134]  Edward T. Bullmore,et al.  Task-induced deactivations during successful paired associates learning: An effect of age but not Alzheimer's disease , 2006, NeuroImage.

[135]  D. Hassabis,et al.  Deconstructing episodic memory with construction , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[136]  Eleanor A Maguire,et al.  Lateral Asymmetry in the Hippocampal Response to the Remoteness of Autobiographical Memories , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[137]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition , 2010, NeuroImage.

[138]  J. Binder,et al.  A Parametric Manipulation of Factors Affecting Task-induced Deactivation in Functional Neuroimaging , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[139]  G. Glover,et al.  Cultural and linguistic influence on neural bases of ‘Theory of Mind’: An fMRI study with Japanese bilinguals , 2006, Brain and Language.

[140]  Josef Perner,et al.  Memory and theory of mind , 2000 .

[141]  Jordan B. Peterson,et al.  Bookworms versus nerds: Exposure to fiction versus non-fiction, divergent associations with social ability, and the simulation of fictional social worlds , 2006 .

[142]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Mentalizing under Uncertainty: Dissociated Neural Responses to Ambiguous and Unambiguous Mental State Inferences , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[143]  R. Corcoran,et al.  The allusive cognitive deficit in paranoia: the case for mental time travel or cognitive self-projection , 2010, Psychological Medicine.

[144]  V. Calhoun,et al.  Lateral differences in the default mode network in healthy controls and patients with schizophrenia , 2010, Human brain mapping.

[145]  Pascale Piolino,et al.  Hippocampal activation for autobiographical memories over the entire lifetime in healthy aged subjects: an fMRI study. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[146]  Robin I. M. Dunbar Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans , 1993, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[147]  R W Cox,et al.  AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. , 1996, Computers and biomedical research, an international journal.

[148]  Brian Levine,et al.  Ventral frontal contribution to self-regulation: Convergence of episodic memory and inhibition , 1999 .

[149]  Brian Levine,et al.  The Functional Neuroanatomy of Episodic and Semantic Autobiographical Remembering: A Prospective Functional MRI Study , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[150]  R. Passingham,et al.  Brain Mechanisms for Inferring Deceit in the Actions of Others , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[151]  Justin M Ream,et al.  Neural Basis of Spontaneous thought Processes , 2004, Cortex.

[152]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[153]  N. Humphrey The Social Function of Intellect , 1976 .

[154]  N C Andreasen,et al.  Remembering the past: two facets of episodic memory explored with positron emission tomography. , 1995, The American journal of psychiatry.

[155]  A. Thiel,et al.  Right amygdalar and temporofrontal activation during autobiographic, but not during fictitious memory retrieval. , 2000, Behavioural neurology.

[156]  Alana T. Wong,et al.  Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[157]  J. Gray,et al.  Allocentric spatial memory activation of the hippocampal formation measured with fMRI. , 2004, Neuropsychology.

[158]  B. Dubreuil,et al.  Paleolithic public goods games: why human culture and cooperation did not evolve in one step , 2010 .

[159]  J. Decety,et al.  A PET Investigation of the Attribution of Intentions with a Nonverbal Task , 2000, NeuroImage.

[160]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Common Blood Flow Changes across Visual Tasks: II. Decreases in Cerebral Cortex , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[161]  Gereon R Fink,et al.  Differential remoteness and emotional tone modulate the neural correlates of autobiographical memory. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[162]  Hans J. Markowitsch,et al.  Psychogenic amnesia – A malady of the constricted self , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.

[163]  Eleanor A. Maguire,et al.  Thoughts, behaviour, and brain dynamics during navigation in the real world , 2006, NeuroImage.

[164]  Keith Oatley,et al.  Exploring the link between reading fiction and empathy: Ruling out individual differences and examining outcomes , 2009 .

[165]  Winfried Rief,et al.  Social performance is more closely associated with theory of mind and autobiographical memory than with psychopathological symptoms in clinically stable patients with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders , 2010, Psychiatry Research.

[166]  Carol V. Ward,et al.  Ecological dominance, social competition, and coalitionary arms races: Why humans evolved extraordinary intelligence. , 2005 .

[167]  E. Tulving,et al.  Consciousness of subjective time in the brain , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[168]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Medial prefrontal cortex subserves diverse forms of self-reflection , 2011, Social neuroscience.

[169]  D. Schacter,et al.  The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: remembering the past and imagining the future , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[170]  Matthew Brett,et al.  The neural basis of autobiographical and semantic memory: New evidence from three PET studies , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[171]  Steve Majerus,et al.  The Neural Basis of Personal Goal Processing When Envisioning Future Events , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[172]  Jay L. Garfield,et al.  Social Cognition, Language Acquisition and The Development of the Theory of Mind , 2001 .

[173]  Keith Oatley,et al.  Joint plans, emotions, and relationships: A diary study of errors , 2006 .

[174]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[175]  James K Rilling,et al.  The neural correlates of theory of mind within interpersonal interactions , 2004, NeuroImage.

[176]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Encoding uncertainty in the hippocampus , 2006, Neural Networks.

[177]  D. Perrett,et al.  Being the target of another’s emotion: a PET study , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[178]  E. Maguire,et al.  Neurodevelopmental Aspects of Spatial Navigation: A Virtual Reality fMRI Study , 2002, NeuroImage.

[179]  C. Atance,et al.  Episodic future thinking , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[180]  B. de Vries,et al.  A Lifetime of Events: Age and Gender Variations in the Life Story , 1996, International journal of aging & human development.

[181]  R. Saxe,et al.  Making sense of another mind: The role of the right temporo-parietal junction , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[182]  Karl K. Szpunar,et al.  On subjective time , 2011, Cortex.

[183]  G K Aguirre,et al.  Neural components of topographical representation. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[184]  M. Hallett,et al.  Modeling other minds , 1995, Neuroreport.

[185]  Olaf Blanke,et al.  Mental time in amnesia: Evidence from bilateral medial temporal damage before and after recovery , 2009, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[186]  J. Lancaster,et al.  Using the talairach atlas with the MNI template , 2001, NeuroImage.

[187]  E. Denkova,et al.  Experiencing past and future personal events: Functional neuroimaging evidence on the neural bases of mental time travel , 2008, Brain and Cognition.

[188]  M. Petrides,et al.  Retrosplenial and hippocampal brain regions in human navigation: complementary functional contributions to the formation and use of cognitive maps , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[189]  Zhong-Lin Lu,et al.  Neural correlates of envisioning emotional events in the near and far future , 2008, NeuroImage.

[190]  Daniel S. Margulies,et al.  Delineating self-referential processing from episodic memory retrieval: Common and dissociable networks , 2010, NeuroImage.

[191]  Meghana Bhatt,et al.  Self-Referential Thinking and Equilibrium as States of Mind in Games: Fmri Evidence , 2005, Games Econ. Behav..

[192]  G. Winocur,et al.  In Search of the Self: A Positron Emission Tomography Study , 1999 .

[193]  C. Frith,et al.  Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[194]  Liang Wang,et al.  Intrinsic connectivity between the hippocampus and posteromedial cortex predicts memory performance in cognitively intact older individuals , 2010, NeuroImage.

[195]  P. Fransson How default is the default mode of brain function? Further evidence from intrinsic BOLD signal fluctuations , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[196]  R. Buckner The role of the hippocampus in prediction and imagination. , 2010, Annual review of psychology.

[197]  Istvan Molnar-Szakacs,et al.  Searching for an integrated self-representation , 2009, Communicative & integrative biology.

[198]  Jiang Xu,et al.  Language in context: emergent features of word, sentence, and narrative comprehension , 2005, NeuroImage.

[199]  E. Phelps,et al.  Neural mechanisms mediating optimism bias , 2007, Nature.

[200]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Knowing where and getting there: a human navigation network. , 1998, Science.

[201]  Josef Perner,et al.  Do visual perspective tasks need theory of mind? , 2006, NeuroImage.

[202]  Suzanne Keen Empathy and the Novel , 2007 .

[203]  D. Rubin,et al.  Brain Activity during Episodic Retrieval of Autobiographical and Laboratory Events: An fMRI Study using a Novel Photo Paradigm , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[204]  Peter Carruthers,et al.  Theories of theories of mind: Frontmatter , 1996 .

[205]  B. Levine,et al.  The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: A meta-analysis , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[206]  E. Denkova,et al.  Neural correlates of remembering/knowing famous people: An event-related fMRI study , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[207]  Hilla Peretz,et al.  The , 1966 .

[208]  Angela M. Uecker,et al.  ALE meta‐analysis: Controlling the false discovery rate and performing statistical contrasts , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[209]  C. Forn,et al.  Memory lateralization with 2 functional MR imaging tasks in patients with lesions in the temporal lobe. , 2006, AJNR. American journal of neuroradiology.

[210]  Masatoshi Itoh,et al.  Thinking of the future and past: the roles of the frontal pole and the medial temporal lobes , 2003, NeuroImage.

[211]  R. Dolan,et al.  An fMRI study of intentional and unintentional (embarrassing) violations of social norms. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[212]  Evelyn C. Ferstl,et al.  What Does the Frontomedian Cortex Contribute to Language Processing: Coherence or Theory of Mind? , 2002, NeuroImage.

[213]  K. Christoff,et al.  Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[214]  E. Tulving Memory and consciousness. , 1985 .

[215]  M. Corballis,et al.  Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind. , 1997, Genetic, social, and general psychology monographs.

[216]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Learning to find your way: a role for the human hippocampal formation , 1996, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[217]  R. Sperling,et al.  What goes down must come up: role of the posteromedial cortices in encoding and retrieval. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[218]  J. Bruner Actual minds, possible worlds , 1985 .

[219]  Eleanor A Maguire,et al.  Aging affects the engagement of the hippocampus during autobiographical memory retrieval. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[220]  S. Scott,et al.  The role of the rostral frontal cortex (area 10) in prospective memory: a lateral versus medial dissociation , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[221]  E. Maguire,et al.  Differential modulation of a common memory retrieval network revealed by positron emission tomography , 1999, Hippocampus.

[222]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Mental navigation along memorized routes activates the hippocampus, precuneus, and insula , 1997, Neuroreport.

[223]  Benjamin J. Shannon,et al.  Molecular, Structural, and Functional Characterization of Alzheimer's Disease: Evidence for a Relationship between Default Activity, Amyloid, and Memory , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[224]  S. Bressler,et al.  Large-scale brain networks in cognition: emerging methods and principles , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[225]  Daniel P. Kennedy,et al.  Differential electrophysiological response during rest, self-referential, and non–self-referential tasks in human posteromedial cortex , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[226]  Elizabeth A. Osuch,et al.  Retrosplenial cortex connectivity in schizophrenia , 2009, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.