Altering Element Interactivity and Intrinsic Cognitive load

As described in Chapter 15, the concept of element interactivity provides a theoretical construct underpinning understanding and learning. Instructional material that is low in element interactivity and consequently low in intrinsic cognitive load requires few working memory resources, because the constituent elements do not interact and can be understood and learned in isolation. Other material that is high in element interactivity and intrinsic cognitive load includes elements that interact and must be processed simultaneously as they cannot be understood and learned as single elements. High element interactivity information is a major category of information that is of interest to cognitive load theory. Processing such information requires working memory resources that may exceed its capacity. That information cannot be easily understood or learned and creates a significant challenge for teachers and instructional designers. How can students learn new, complex, high element interactivity information if the processing demand is greater than working memory capacity?

[1]  J. Sweller,et al.  Cognitive Load Theory and Complex Learning: Recent Developments and Future Directions , 2005 .

[2]  M. Chi,et al.  Eliciting Self‐Explanations Improves Understanding , 1994 .

[3]  John R. Anderson ACT: A simple theory of complex cognition. , 1996 .

[4]  R. Catrambone The subgoal learning model: Creating better examples so that students can solve novel problems. , 1998 .

[5]  John Sweller,et al.  Interactions between the isolated–interactive elements effect and levels of learner expertise: experimental evidence from an accountancy class , 2010 .

[6]  F. Paas,et al.  Variability of Worked Examples and Transfer of Geometrical Problem-Solving Skills: A Cognitive-Load Approach , 1994 .

[7]  Paul Ayres Using subjective measures to detect variations of intrinsic cognitive load within problems , 2006 .

[8]  Dominique Sluijsmans,et al.  Toward a Synthesis of Cognitive Load Theory, Four-Component Instructional Design, and Self-Directed Learning , 2009 .

[9]  J. Michael Spector,et al.  Handbook of Research on Educational Communications and Technology, 3rd Edition , 2012 .

[10]  Jill L. Quilici,et al.  Role of examples in how students learn to categorize statistics word problems. , 1996 .

[11]  P. Kirschner,et al.  Optimizing the number of steps in learning tasks for complex skills. , 2005, The British journal of educational psychology.

[12]  John Sweller,et al.  The impact of sequencing and prior knowledge on learning mathematics through spreadsheet applications , 2005 .

[13]  F. Paas,et al.  Cognitive Architecture and Instructional Design , 1998 .

[14]  Charles M. Reigeluth,et al.  Order, first step to mastery: An introduction to sequencing in instructional design. , 2007 .

[15]  R. Catrambone,et al.  Can learning from molar and modular worked examples be enhanced by providing instructional explanations and prompting self-explanations? * , 2006 .

[16]  Paul A. Kirschner,et al.  Ten Steps to Complex Learning: A Systematic Approach to Four-Component Instructional Design , 2007 .

[17]  R. Atkinson,et al.  Structuring the Transition From Example Study to Problem Solving in Cognitive Skill Acquisition: A Cognitive Load Perspective , 2003 .

[18]  Frank E. Ritter,et al.  In order to learn : how the sequence of topics influences learning , 2007 .

[19]  Fred Paas,et al.  High versus low contextual interference in simulation-based training of troubleshooting skills: effects on transfer performance and invested mental effort , 1998 .

[20]  Richard Catrambone,et al.  Designing Instructional Examples to Reduce Intrinsic Cognitive Load: Molar versus Modular Presentation of Solution Procedures , 2004 .

[21]  Paul A. Kirschner,et al.  Just-in-time information presentation: Improving learning a troubleshooting skill. , 2006 .

[22]  John Sweller,et al.  Efficiency in Learning: Evidence-Based Guidelines to Manage Cognitive Load , 2005 .

[23]  F. Paas,et al.  Teaching complex rather than simple tasks: balancing intrinsic and germane load to enhance transfer of learning , 2006 .

[24]  Michelene T. H. Chi,et al.  Expertise in Problem Solving. , 1981 .

[25]  P. Kirschner,et al.  Taking the Load Off a Learner's Mind: Instructional Design for Complex Learning , 2003 .

[26]  J. Merriënboer,et al.  Whole-Task Models in Education , 2007 .

[27]  P. Chandler,et al.  Assimilating complex information , 2002 .

[28]  Patricia D. Mautone,et al.  Pictorial aids for learning by doing in a multimedia geology simulation game. , 2002 .

[29]  Paul Ayres Systematic Mathematical Errors and Cognitive Load. , 2001, Contemporary educational psychology.

[30]  Richard E. Clark,et al.  Blueprints for complex learning: The 4C/ID-model , 2002 .

[31]  Richard E Mayer,et al.  Fostering understanding of multimedia messages through pre-training: evidence for a two-stage theory of mental model construction. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[32]  R. Sternberg Advances in the psychology of human intelligence , 1982 .

[33]  Matthew W. Lewis,et al.  Self-Explonations: How Students Study and Use Examples in Learning to Solve Problems , 1989, Cogn. Sci..

[34]  Paul Ayres Impact of Reducing Intrinsic Cognitive Load on Learning in a Mathematical Domain. , 2006 .