Humans, rats and mice show species-specific adaptations to sensory statistics in categorisation behaviour

One key aspect of animal intelligence is the ability to find, represent and update knowledge of statistical regularities in the sensory environment, in service of adaptive behaviour. This allows animals to build appropriate priors, in order to disambiguate noisy inputs, make predictions and act more efficiently. Here, we compare human, rat, and mouse performance on a 2-alternative-forced-choice sound categorisation task. Importantly, we manipulate the stimulus statistics without affecting the overall weight of each category, and we investigate decision-making in such distinct ‘statistical contexts’ associated with different stimulus prior probabilities. Humans, rats, and mice develop a sensory-dependent bias that increases reward to a near-optimal level. We also explore how flexible animals are in learning different types of statistics, without any explicit cue, when switching from one prior probability to another. All species can adapt to a new underlying distribution by gradually shifting their psychometric behaviour after the switch to a new statistical context, with mice showing the slowest rate of adaptation. Despite the overall sensory-dependent bias that is similar across species, humans versus rodents show distinct trial-to-trial learning updates. Rodents show an overall win-stay choice bias, with rats incorporating decision confidence more strongly than mice. Humans, on the other hand, show a more complex learning update, and more indicative of utilising detailed sensory statistics. We propose two classes of computational models, a boundary learning (discriminative) and a stimulus-category learning (generative) model. We show how they can account for the optimal exploitation of the sensory statistics, and predict the observed species-specific trial-to-trial updates.

[1]  Yu Xin,et al.  A cortical circuit mechanism for structural knowledge-based flexible sensorimotor decision-making , 2021, Neuron.

[2]  Ji Hyun Bak,et al.  Extracting the dynamics of behavior in sensory decision-making experiments , 2021, Neuron.

[3]  Nicholas A. Roy,et al.  Mice alternate between discrete strategies during perceptual decision-making , 2020, bioRxiv.

[4]  M. Diamond,et al.  Dynamics of history-dependent perceptual judgment , 2020, Nature Communications.

[5]  Anne E. Urai,et al.  Reinforcement biases subsequent perceptual decisions when confidence is low, a widespread behavioral phenomenon , 2020, eLife.

[6]  Brynn E. Sherman,et al.  The prevalence and importance of statistical learning in human cognition and behavior , 2020, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[7]  Eelke Spaak,et al.  A Bayesian and efficient observer model explains concurrent attractive and repulsive history biases in visual perception , 2020, bioRxiv.

[8]  Fanny Cazettes,et al.  Standardized and reproducible measurement of decision-making in mice , 2020, bioRxiv.

[9]  Joel Nothman,et al.  SciPy 1.0-Fundamental Algorithms for Scientific Computing in Python , 2019, ArXiv.

[10]  Y. Zhang,et al.  Sensory-to-Category Transformation via Dynamic Reorganization of Ensemble Structures in Mouse Auditory Cortex , 2019, Neuron.

[11]  Yuan Zhang,et al.  Causal contributions of parietal cortex to perceptual decision-making during stimulus categorization , 2019, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  J. Gray,et al.  PsychoPy2: Experiments in behavior made easy , 2019, Behavior research methods.

[13]  Xiaoqin Wang,et al.  Optimal features for auditory categorization , 2018, bioRxiv.

[14]  Charles D. Kopec,et al.  Posterior parietal cortex represents sensory history and mediates its effects on behaviour , 2017, Nature.

[15]  Justin L. Gardner,et al.  Adaptable history biases in human perceptual decisions , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[16]  Xue-Xin Wei,et al.  A Bayesian observer model constrained by efficient coding can explain 'anti-Bayesian' percepts , 2015, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Efficient Sensory Encoding and Bayesian Inference with Heterogeneous Neural Populations , 2014, Neural Computation.

[18]  Gonçalo Lopes,et al.  Bonsai: an event-based framework for processing and controlling data streams , 2014, bioRxiv.

[19]  W. Freeman,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews the Bayesian Brain: Phantom Percepts Resolve Sensory Uncertainty , 2022 .

[20]  M. Diamond,et al.  Tactile perception and working memory in rats and humans , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[21]  S. Kelly,et al.  Internal and External Influences on the Rate of Sensory Evidence Accumulation in the Human Brain , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  David D. Cox,et al.  Making a Science of Model Search: Hyperparameter Optimization in Hundreds of Dimensions for Vision Architectures , 2013, ICML.

[23]  David Raposo,et al.  Multisensory Decision-Making in Rats and Humans , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  Andrew D. Zaharia,et al.  The Detection of Visual Contrast in the Behaving Mouse , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  John M. Pearson,et al.  Posterior cingulate cortex: adapting behavior to a changing world , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Dynamic Integration of Reward and Stimulus Information in Perceptual Decision-Making , 2011, PloS one.

[27]  József Fiser,et al.  Spontaneous Cortical Activity Reveals Hallmarks of an Optimal Internal Model of the Environment , 2011, Science.

[28]  Peter Neri,et al.  How inherently noisy is human sensory processing? , 2010, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[29]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Integration of Sensory and Reward Information during Perceptual Decision-Making in Lateral Intraparietal Cortex (LIP) of the Macaque Monkey , 2010, PloS one.

[30]  Brian E. Russ,et al.  Prefrontal Activity Predicts Monkeys' Decisions During an Auditory Category Task , 2009, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[31]  Gonzalo H. Otazu,et al.  Engaging in an auditory task suppresses responses in auditory cortex , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  Philip Holmes,et al.  Can Monkeys Choose Optimally When Faced with Noisy Stimuli and Unequal Rewards? , 2009, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[33]  Y. Cohen,et al.  Neural and behavioral correlates of auditory categorization , 2007, Hearing Research.

[34]  R. Weisman,et al.  Auditory category perception as a natural cognitive activity in songbirds , 2007 .

[35]  Tony Jebara,et al.  Machine learning: Discriminative and generative , 2006 .

[36]  E. Mercado,et al.  Auditory categorization of complex sounds by rats (Rattus norvegicus). , 2005, Journal of comparative psychology.

[37]  D. Knill,et al.  The Bayesian brain: the role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.

[38]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. , 2001, Psychological review.

[39]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Can sequence learning be implicit? New evidence with the process dissociation procedure , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[40]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Natural image statistics and neural representation. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[41]  M. Hausberger,et al.  Categorization in birdsong: from behavioural to neuronal responses , 1995, Behavioural Processes.

[42]  K. H. Britten,et al.  Responses of neurons in macaque MT to stochastic motion signals , 1993, Visual Neuroscience.

[43]  J. Movshon,et al.  The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[44]  John R. Anderson,et al.  The Adaptive Nature of Human Categorization , 1991 .

[45]  H. Simon,et al.  Rationality in Psychology and Economics , 1986 .

[46]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .