Illuminating Mental Representations Through Speech and Gesture

Can the gestures people produce when describing algebra word problems offer insight into their mental representations of the problems? Twenty adults were asked to describe six word problems about constant change, and then to talk aloud as they solved the problems. Two problems depicted continuous change, two depicted discrete change, and two depicted change that could be construed as either continuous or discrete. Participants' verbal and gestured descriptions of the problems often incorporated information about manner of change. However, the information conveyed in gesture was not always the same as the information conveyed in speech. Participants' problem representations, as expressed in speech and gesture, were systematically related to their problem solutions. When gesture reinforced the representation expressed in the spoken description, participants were very likely to solve the problem using a strategy compatible with that representation—much more likely than when gesture did not reinforce the spoken description. The results indicate that gesture and speech together provide a better index of mental representation than speech alone.

[1]  P. Johnson-Laird Mental models , 1989 .

[2]  M. Alibali,et al.  Gesture-Speech Mismatch and Mechanisms of Learning: What the Hands Reveal about a Child′s State of Mind , 1993, Cognitive Psychology.

[3]  M. Alibali,et al.  Transitions in concept acquisition: using the hand to read the mind. , 1993, Psychological review.

[4]  Michelle Perry,et al.  Knowledge in transition: Adults' developing understanding of a principle of physical causality , 1997 .

[5]  M. Bassok,et al.  Judging a book by its cover: Interpretative effects of content on problem-solving transfer , 1995, Memory & cognition.

[6]  S Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Knowledge conveyed in gesture is not tied to the hands. , 1998, Child development.

[7]  M. Alibali How children change their minds: strategy change can be gradual or abrupt. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[8]  Daniel L. Schwartz,et al.  Shuttling Between Depictive Models and Abstract Rules: Induction and Fallback , 1996, Cogn. Sci..

[9]  H. Simon,et al.  Why are some problems hard? Evidence from Tower of Hanoi , 1985, Cognitive Psychology.

[10]  Denis Newman,et al.  Telling what they know: The role of gesture and language in children's science explanations , 1993 .

[11]  Herbert A. Simon,et al.  Understanding written problem instructions. , 1974 .

[12]  S. Goldin-Meadow,et al.  The mismatch between gesture and speech as an index of transitional knowledge , 1986, Cognition.

[13]  Marsha C. Lovett,et al.  Task representations, strategy variability, and base-rate neglect , 1999 .

[14]  W. Kintsch,et al.  Strategies of discourse comprehension , 1983 .

[15]  Herbert A Simon,et al.  The understanding process: Problem isomorphs , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[16]  Lee W. Gregg,et al.  Knowledge and cognition , 1974 .

[17]  Mary Jo Kane,et al.  Young children's mental models determine analogical transfer across problems with a common goal structure * , 1986 .

[18]  R. Siegler,et al.  Mechanisms of cognitive development. , 1989, Annual review of psychology.

[19]  Miriam Bassok,et al.  OBJECT-BASED REPRESENTATIONS : TRANSFER BETWEEN CASES OF CONTINUOUS AND DISCRETE MODELS OF CHANGE , 1995 .

[20]  D. McNeill Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal about Thought , 1992 .

[21]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Protocol Analysis: Verbal Reports as Data , 1984 .

[22]  Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Transitional knowledge in the acquisition of concepts , 1988 .

[23]  W Kintsch,et al.  Understanding and solving word arithmetic problems. , 1985, Psychological review.