The roles of vision and proprioception in the planning of reaching movements.

While vision and proprioception can both provide information about arm configuration prior to movement, substantial evidence suggests that each modality is used for different stages of the planning process. In this chapter, we provide support for the idea that vision is mainly used to define the trajectory and the kinematics of reaching movements. Proprioception appears to be critical in the transformation of this plan into the motor commands sent to the arm muscles.

[1]  S Gilman,et al.  Kinematic effects of deafferentation and cerebellar ablation. , 1976, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[2]  S. Freedman The Neuropsychology of spatially oriented behavior , 1968 .

[3]  J C Rothwell,et al.  Manual motor performance in a deafferented man. , 1982, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[4]  A. G. Feldman,et al.  Recent tests of the equilibrium-point hypothesis (lambda model). , 1998, Motor control.

[5]  James Gordon,et al.  Organization of voluntary movement , 1991, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[6]  J. Vercher,et al.  Target and hand position information in the online control of goal-directed arm movements , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  C. Sherrington,et al.  VIII. Experiments upon the influence of sensory nerves upon movement and nutrition of the limbs. Preliminary communication , 1985, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London.

[8]  B. Day,et al.  Voluntary modification of automatic arm movements evoked by motion of a visual target , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  E. Brenner,et al.  Fast corrections of movements with a computer mouse. , 2003, Spatial vision.

[10]  J. J. Denier van der Gon,et al.  Integration of proprioceptive and visual position-information , 1999 .

[11]  J. Cooke,et al.  Initial agonist burst is modified by perturbations preceding movement , 1986, Brain Research.

[12]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Touching the phantom limb , 1995, Nature.

[13]  C. Atkeson,et al.  Kinematic features of unrestrained vertical arm movements , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[14]  C. Ghez,et al.  Loss of proprioception produces deficits in interjoint coordination. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  J. Vercher,et al.  Online control of the direction of rapid reaching movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  J. Lackner,et al.  The role of brachial muscle spindle signals in assignment of visual direction. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  D. Rosenbaum Human movement initiation: specification of arm, direction, and extent. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[18]  C. Gielen,et al.  Relation between EMG activation patterns and kinematic properties of aimed arm movements. , 1985, Journal of motor behavior.

[19]  M. Latash,et al.  Organizing principles for single joint movements. III. Speed-insensitive strategy as a default. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  T. Flash,et al.  The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[21]  R. Schmidt,et al.  Utilization of Sensory Information for Motor Control , 1990 .

[22]  J. Lackner,et al.  Influence of vision on vibration-induced illusions of limb movement , 1984, Experimental Neurology.

[23]  J. Gordon,et al.  Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. II. Effects of visual information on accuracy. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Systematic changes in motor cortex cell activity with arm posture during directional isometric force generation. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Are arm trajectories planned in kinematic or dynamic coordinates? An adaptation study , 1995, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  T. Flash,et al.  Arm Trajectory Modifications During Reaching Towards Visual Targets , 1991, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  K. Boivin,et al.  Exploring the Limits of Peripheral Vision for the Control of Movement , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.

[28]  J. Gordon,et al.  Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. I. Spatial errors. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Vectorial coding of movement: vision, proprioception, or both? , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  G E Alexander,et al.  Neural representations of the target (goal) of visually guided arm movements in three motor areas of the monkey. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  R. Sainburg,et al.  The effect of target modality on visual and proprioceptive contributions to the control of movement distance , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Sensorimotor representations for pointing to targets in three-dimensional space. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  J. Cooke,et al.  Initial agonist burst duration depends on movement amplitude , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  S. A. Wallace,et al.  An empirical note on attaining a spatial target after distorting the initial conditions of movement via muscle vibration. , 1984, Journal of motor behavior.

[35]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Optimal response of eye and hand motor systems in pointing at a visual target , 1979, Biological Cybernetics.

[36]  J. Paillard,et al.  Active and passive movements in the calibration of position sense , 1968 .

[37]  F. W. Mott,et al.  Experiments on the Influence of Sensory Nerves upon Movement and Nutrition of the Limbs. , 1895 .

[38]  R. Schmidt,et al.  Changes in limb dynamics during the practice of rapid arm movements. , 1989, Journal of biomechanics.

[39]  Y. Lamarre,et al.  Rapid elbow flexion in the absence of proprioceptive and cutaneous feedback. , 1987, Human neurobiology.

[40]  M. Latash,et al.  Effects of altering initial position on movement direction and extent. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  R. Sainburg,et al.  Differential contributions of vision and proprioception to movement accuracy , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Neuronal population coding of movement direction. , 1986, Science.

[43]  M. Latash,et al.  Testing hypotheses and the advancement of science: recent attempts to falsify the equilibrium point hypothesis , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  C. Ghez,et al.  The control of rapid limb movement in the cat , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[45]  T. Ebner,et al.  Temporal encoding of movement kinematics in the discharge of primate primary motor and premotor neurons. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  W. Prinz,et al.  Relationships Between Perception and Action: Current Approaches , 1990 .

[47]  F. Lestienne Effects of inertial load and velocity on the braking process of voluntary limb movements , 1979, Experimental Brain Research.

[48]  David C Knill,et al.  Visual Feedback Control of Hand Movements , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Characteristics of motor programs underlying arm movements in monkeys. , 1979, Journal of neurophysiology.

[50]  G. Gottlieb,et al.  The neural control of single degree-of-freedom elbow movements , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[51]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.

[52]  G. Gottlieb,et al.  EMG responses to an unexpected load in fast movements are delayed with an increase in the expected movement time. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[53]  R L Sainburg,et al.  Control of limb dynamics in normal subjects and patients without proprioception. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[54]  G. Fullerton Psychology and physiology. , 1896 .

[55]  J Paillard,et al.  Fast and slow feedback loops for the visual correction of spatial errors in a pointing task: a reappraisal. , 1996, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[56]  D. Vicario,et al.  The control of rapid limb movement in the cat II. Scaling of isometric force adjustments , 1978, Experimental brain research.

[57]  A P Georgopoulos,et al.  Online visual control of the arm. , 1998, Novartis Foundation symposium.

[58]  C. Kukulka,et al.  Relationship between muscle length and moment arm on EMG activity of human triceps surae muscle. , 2004, Journal of electromyography and kinesiology : official journal of the International Society of Electrophysiological Kinesiology.

[59]  S. Scott,et al.  Changes in motor cortex activity during reaching movements with similar hand paths but different arm postures. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[60]  J. Cooke,et al.  Responses to force perturbations preceding voluntary human arm movements , 1981, Brain Research.

[61]  David A. Rosenbaum,et al.  Movement speed effects on limb position drift , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[62]  Daniel M. Corcos,et al.  Nonlinear control of movement distance at the human elbow , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[63]  D. Hoffman,et al.  Muscle and movement representations in the primary motor cortex. , 1999, Science.

[64]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Role of the posterior parietal cortex in updating reaching movements to a visual target , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[65]  R. Daroff,et al.  Clinical Neurology for Psychiatrists , 1995, Neurology.

[66]  R. J. van Beers,et al.  Integration of proprioceptive and visual position-information: An experimentally supported model. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[67]  Fabrice R. Sarlegna,et al.  Impairment of online control of reaching movements with aging: A double-step study , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.

[68]  A. Riehle,et al.  Monkey primary motor and premotor cortex: single-cell activity related to prior information about direction and extent of an intended movement. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[69]  J. Vercher,et al.  Internally driven control of reaching movements: A study on a proprioceptively deafferented subject , 2006, Brain Research Bulletin.

[70]  D. Rosenbaum,et al.  Limb position drift: implications for control of posture and movement. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[71]  C. Ghez,et al.  Trajectory control in targeted force impulses , 1987, Experimental Brain Research.

[72]  J R Flanagan,et al.  Trajectory adaptation to a nonlinear visuomotor transformation: evidence of motion planning in visually perceived space. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[73]  P Cordo,et al.  Proprioceptive coordination of discrete movement sequences: mechanism and generality. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[74]  J. Wann,et al.  Does limb proprioception drift? , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[75]  G. Gauthier,et al.  Oculo-manual tracking of visual targets in monkey: role of the arm afferent information in the control of arm and eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[76]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Differential influence of vision and proprioception on control of movement distance , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[77]  J. Sanes,et al.  Motor control in humans with large-fiber sensory neuropathy. , 1985, Human neurobiology.

[78]  Robert Sessions Woodworth,et al.  THE ACCURACY OF VOLUNTARY MOVEMENT , 1899 .

[79]  P. Viviani,et al.  Pointing errors reflect biases in the perception of the initial hand position. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[80]  C. Bard,et al.  Control of single-joint movements in deafferented patients: evidence for amplitude coding rather than position control , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[81]  D. Pélisson,et al.  From Eye to Hand: Planning Goal-directed Movements , 1998, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[82]  E. Taub,et al.  Deafferentation in monkeys: Pointing at a target without visual feedback , 1975, Experimental Neurology.

[83]  R L Sainburg,et al.  Intersegmental dynamics are controlled by sequential anticipatory, error correction, and postural mechanisms. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[84]  P. Morasso Spatial control of arm movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[85]  J. McIntyre,et al.  Kinematic and dynamic processes for the control of pointing movements in humans revealed by short-term exposure to microgravity , 2005, Neuroscience.

[86]  M S Graziano,et al.  Coding the location of the arm by sight. , 2000, Science.

[87]  C. Ghez,et al.  Discrete and continuous planning of hand movements and isometric force trajectories , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[88]  A. G. Feldman,et al.  Recent Tests of the Equilibrium-Point Hypothesis (λ Model) , 1998 .

[89]  Romeo Chua,et al.  Discrete vs. continuous visual control of manual aiming , 1991 .

[90]  J. D. Cooke,et al.  Initial agonist burst duration changes with movement amplitude in a deafferented patient , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[91]  Nicholas P. Holmes,et al.  Visual bias of unseen hand position with a mirror: spatial and temporal factors , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[92]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  Multisensory Integration during Motor Planning , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[93]  J. Soechting,et al.  Modification of trajectory of a pointing movement in response to a change in target location. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[94]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[95]  Howard Poizner,et al.  Influence of movement speed on accuracy and coordination of reaching movements to memorized targets in three-dimensional space in a deafferented subject , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[96]  M. Hoy,et al.  The role of intersegmental dynamics during rapid limb oscillations. , 1986, Journal of biomechanics.

[97]  H. Pick,et al.  Visual capture produced by prism spectacles , 1965 .

[98]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Viewing the hand prior to movement improves accuracy of pointing performed toward the unseen contralateral hand , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.