The statistical structure of natural light patterns determines perceived light intensity.

The same target luminance in different contexts can elicit markedly different perceptions of brightness, a fact that has long puzzled vision scientists. Here we test the proposal that the visual system encodes not luminance as such but rather the statistical relationship of a particular luminance to all possible luminance values experienced in natural contexts during evolution. This statistical conception of vision was validated by using a database of natural scenes in which we could determine the probability distribution functions of co-occurring target and contextual luminance values. The distribution functions obtained in this way predict target brightness in response to a variety of challenging stimuli, thus explaining these otherwise puzzling percepts. That brightness is determined by the statistics of natural light patterns implies that the relevant neural circuitry is specifically organized to generate these probabilistic responses.

[1]  A. Gilchrist,et al.  An anchoring theory of lightness perception. , 1999 .

[2]  J. Schirillo,et al.  T-junctions in inhomogeneous surrounds , 2000, Vision Research.

[3]  U. Grenander Elements of Pattern Theory , 1996 .

[4]  Dale Purves,et al.  Perceiving the intensity of light. , 2004, Psychological review.

[5]  T. Albright,et al.  Contextual influences on visual processing. , 2002, Annual review of neuroscience.

[6]  P. Whittle Brightness, discriminability and the “Crispening Effect” , 1992, Vision Research.

[7]  D. Wilkin,et al.  Neuron , 2001, Brain Research.

[8]  M. McCourt,et al.  A multiscale spatial filtering account of the White effect, simultaneous brightness contrast and grating induction , 1999, Vision Research.

[9]  L. Fisher Vitamin A problems in marine research , 1962, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series A. Mathematical and Physical Sciences.

[10]  Bernt Schiele,et al.  Computer vision systems , 2003, Machine Vision and Applications.

[11]  Bruno A. Olshausen,et al.  Book Review , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  David J. Heeger,et al.  Neuronal correlates of perception in early visual cortex , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  Allen R. Hanson,et al.  Computer Vision Systems , 1978 .

[14]  O. L. Zangwill,et al.  Current Problems in Animal Behavior , 1964 .

[15]  J. Frisby,et al.  The Role of 3-D Surface Slope in a Lightness/Brightness Effect , 1997, Vision Research.

[16]  Michael J. Berry,et al.  The Neural Code of the Retina , 1999, Neuron.

[17]  Adrienne L. Fairhall,et al.  Efficiency and ambiguity in an adaptive neural code , 2001, Nature.

[18]  M. Lévesque Perception , 1986, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.

[19]  P. Howe,et al.  A comment on the Anderson (1997), the Todorović (1997), and the Ross and Pessoa (2000) explanations of White's effect. , 2001, Perception.

[20]  D. Long Probabilistic Models of the Brain. , 2002 .

[21]  M. Paradiso,et al.  Neural Correlates of Perceived Brightness in the Retina, Lateral Geniculate Nucleus, and Striate Cortex , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  E. Adelson Perceptual organization and the judgment of brightness. , 1993, Science.

[23]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Representation of Color Stimuli in Awake Macaque Primary Visual Cortex , 2003, Neuron.

[24]  D. Macleod,et al.  Optimal nonlinear codes for the perception of natural colours , 2001, Network.

[25]  O. L. Zangwill,et al.  Current problems in animal behaviour , 1962 .

[26]  Arash Yazdanbakhsh,et al.  Munker–White-Like Illusions without T-Junctions , 2002, Perception.

[27]  B. Anderson Perceptual Organization and White's Illusion , 2003, Perception.

[28]  D. Purves,et al.  Why we see what we do : an empirical theory of vision , 2003 .

[29]  A. Gilchrist,et al.  The critical role of relative luminance relations in White's effect and grating induction , 1995, Vision Research.

[30]  J. H. Hateren,et al.  Independent component filters of natural images compared with simple cells in primary visual cortex , 1998 .

[31]  Dale Purves,et al.  Image/source statistics of surfaces in natural scenes , 2003, Network.

[32]  M. Gazzaniga,et al.  The new cognitive neurosciences , 2000 .

[33]  J L Gallant,et al.  Sparse coding and decorrelation in primary visual cortex during natural vision. , 2000, Science.

[34]  R C Reid,et al.  Efficient Coding of Natural Scenes in the Lateral Geniculate Nucleus: Experimental Test of a Computational Theory , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  Michael J. Berry,et al.  Adaptation of retinal processing to image contrast and spatial scale , 1997, Nature.

[36]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Natural image statistics and neural representation. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[37]  D. Knill,et al.  Apparent surface curvature affects lightness perception , 1991, Nature.

[38]  D. Todorović Lightness and Junctions , 1997, Perception.

[39]  M. White,et al.  A New Effect of Pattern on Perceived Lightness , 1979, Perception.

[40]  W. D. Ross,et al.  Lightness from contrast: A selective integration model , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[41]  B. Spehar,et al.  Induced Effects of Backgrounds and Foregrounds in Three-Dimensional Configurations: The Role of T-Junctions , 1997, Perception.

[42]  Dale Purves,et al.  A statistical explanation of visual space , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[43]  Colin W G Clifford,et al.  Induction in Variants of White's Effect: Common or Separate Mechanisms? , 2002, Perception.