Imitative flexibility and the development of cultural learning

Two studies test the hypothesis that imitative fidelity is influenced by cues to interpret behavior as instrumental versus conventional. Study 1 (N=57, 4-5-yr-olds) manipulated non-verbal cues (start- and end-states of action sequences) and Study 2 (N=211, 4-6-yr-olds) manipulated verbal cues to examine the effects of information about instrumental versus conventional goals on imitative fidelity. Imitative fidelity was highest (Studies 1 and 2), innovation was lowest (Study 1), and difference detection was more accurate (Study 2) when cued with information about conventional rather than instrumental behavior. The results provide novel insight into the kinds of information children use to adjudicate between instrumental and conventional behavior.

[1]  Joseph Henrich,et al.  The evolution of costly displays, cooperation and religion: credibility enhancing displays and their implications for cultural evolution , 2009 .

[2]  Harvey Whitehouse,et al.  Task-specific effects of ostracism on imitative fidelity in early childhood☆ , 2014 .

[3]  M. Nielsen,et al.  The perpetuation of ritualistic actions as revealed by young children's transmission of normative behavior , 2015 .

[4]  A. Whiten,et al.  Imitative learning of artificial fruit processing in children (Homo sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). , 1996, Journal of comparative psychology.

[5]  Andrew Whiten,et al.  Integrating the study of conformity and culture in humans and nonhuman animals. , 2012, Psychological bulletin.

[6]  Christopher L. Suhler,et al.  When It’s an Error to Mirror , 2011, Psychological science.

[7]  Harriet Over,et al.  Priming third-party ostracism increases affiliative imitation in children. , 2009, Developmental science.

[8]  Lydia M. Hopper,et al.  Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[9]  Andrew G. Young,et al.  The hidden structure of overimitation , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[10]  Harlene Hayne,et al.  The effect of event structure on imitation in infancy : Practice makes perfect ? , 1996 .

[11]  L. Schulz,et al.  God does not play dice: causal determinism and preschoolers' causal inferences. , 2006, Child development.

[12]  F. Staal Rules Without Meaning: Ritual, Mantras and the Human Sciences , 1989 .

[13]  Tanya L. Chartrand,et al.  I Am Too Just Like You , 2008, Psychological science.

[14]  A. Henderson,et al.  Language matters: thirteen-month-olds understand that the language a speaker uses constrains conventionality. , 2013, Developmental psychology.

[15]  J. Henrich,et al.  Culture–gene coevolution, norm-psychology and the emergence of human prosociality , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  Stephen C. Want,et al.  How do children ape? Applying concepts from the study of non-human primates to the developmental study of 'imitation' in children , 2002 .

[17]  B. Kenward,et al.  Over-imitation is better explained by norm learning than by distorted causal learning , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[18]  Geoffrey Bird,et al.  ‘Goals’ are not an integral component of imitation , 2010, Cognition.

[19]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Conformity to peer pressure in preschool children. , 2011, Child development.

[20]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Children’s imitation of causal action sequences is influenced by statistical and pedagogical evidence , 2011, Cognition.

[21]  S. Carey,et al.  Reasoning about ‘irrational’ actions: When intentional movements cannot be explained, the movements themselves are seen as the goal , 2013, Cognition.

[22]  I. Užgiris Two Functions of Imitation During Infancy , 1981 .

[23]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Copying results and copying actions in the process of social learning: chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens) , 2005, Animal Cognition.

[24]  E. Durkheim FROM THE ELEMENTARY FORMS OF THE RELIGIOUS LIFE , 1996, The New Economic Sociology.

[25]  Intentions L. Woodward Infants' Grasp of Others' , 2009 .

[26]  T. Kushnir,et al.  Social context effects in 2- and 4-year-olds' selective versus faithful imitation. , 2014, Developmental psychology.

[27]  M. Nielsen,et al.  Copying actions and copying outcomes: social learning through the second year. , 2006, Developmental psychology.

[28]  T. Chartrand,et al.  Using Nonconscious Behavioral Mimicry to Create Affiliation and Rapport , 2003, Psychological science.

[29]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Developmental psychology: Rational imitation in preverbal infants , 2002, Nature.

[30]  M. Nielsen,et al.  Overimitation in Kalahari Bushman Children and the Origins of Human Cultural Cognition , 2010, Psychological science.

[31]  D. Haun,et al.  Majority influence in children and other animals , 2013, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Young children's understanding of the context-relativity of normative rules in conventional games. , 2009, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[33]  György Gergely,et al.  Sylvia's Recipe: The Role of Imitation and Pedagogy in the Transmission of Cultural Knowledge , 2006 .

[34]  Andrew Whiten,et al.  Imitation of causally opaque versus causally transparent tool use by 3- and 5-year-old children , 2007 .

[35]  Emma Flynn,et al.  Whom do children copy? Model-based biases in social learning , 2013 .

[36]  M. Gluckman Rituals of Rebellion in South-East Africa , 1954 .

[37]  P. Harris,et al.  Stick to the script: The effect of witnessing multiple actors on children’s imitation , 2013, Cognition.

[38]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Imitative learning of actions on objects by children, chimpanzees, and enculturated chimpanzees. , 1993, Child development.

[39]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Use of social information in the problem solving of orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) and human children (Homo sapiens). , 1995, Journal of comparative psychology.

[40]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[41]  S. Preston,et al.  Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[42]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Young children enforce social norms selectively depending on the violator’s group affiliation , 2012, Cognition.

[43]  G. Simcock,et al.  The effect of social engagement on 24-month-olds' imitation from live and televised models. , 2008, Developmental science.

[44]  Cecilia Heyes,et al.  Evolution, development and intentional control of imitation , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[45]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Young Children Enforce Social Norms , 2012 .

[46]  Kathleen H Corriveau,et al.  Preschoolers (sometimes) defer to the majority in making simple perceptual judgments. , 2010, Developmental psychology.

[47]  A. Woodward Infants' Grasp of Others' Intentions , 2009, Current directions in psychological science.

[48]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  Report Majority-biased Transmission in Chimpanzees and Human Children, but Not Orangutans , 2022 .

[49]  J. Henrich,et al.  Culture-Gene Coevolution, Large-Scale Cooperation, and the Shaping of Human Social Psychology , 2013 .

[50]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Processes of social learning in the tool use of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). , 1993, Journal of comparative psychology.

[51]  M. Tomasello,et al.  How selective are 3-year-olds in imitating novel linguistic material? , 2013, Developmental psychology.

[52]  C. Heyes,et al.  General processes, rather than "goals," explain imitation errors. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[53]  Mark Nielsen,et al.  Imitation in young children: when who gets copied is more important than what gets copied. , 2011, Developmental psychology.

[54]  E. Markman,et al.  Prior experiences and perceived efficacy influence 3-year-olds' imitation. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[55]  J. Osofsky Handbook of infant development , 1979 .

[56]  H. Hayne,et al.  Developmental changes in imitation from television during infancy. , 1999, Child development.

[57]  R. Rappaport Ritual and Religion in the Making of Humanity: Enactments of meaning , 1999 .

[58]  Chris Moore,et al.  Young children overimitate in third-party contexts. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[59]  Cristine H. Legare,et al.  Priming randomness increases the evaluation of ritual efficacy , 2013, CogSci.

[60]  J. R. Landis,et al.  The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. , 1977, Biometrics.

[61]  C. Legare,et al.  The Evolution and Ontogeny of Ritual , 2015 .

[62]  Harvey Whitehouse,et al.  The Coexistence Problem in Psychology, Anthropology, and Evolutionary Theory , 2011, Human Development.

[63]  K. Rosengren,et al.  The coexistence of natural and supernatural explanations across cultures and development. , 2012, Child development.

[64]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  The sources of normativity: young children's awareness of the normative structure of games. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[65]  G. Csibra,et al.  Beyond rational imitation: learning arbitrary means actions from communicative demonstrations. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[66]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  The Importance of Imitation for Theories of Social‐Cognitive Development , 2010 .

[67]  Frank C Keil,et al.  The scope and limits of overimitation in the transmission of artefact culture , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[68]  Alex Mesoudi,et al.  How cultural evolutionary theory can inform social psychology and vice versa. , 2009, Psychological review.

[69]  Cristine H. Legare,et al.  Cognitive consequences and constraints on reasoning about ritual , 2013 .

[70]  P. Harris Trusting What You’re Told , 2012 .

[71]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[72]  C. Patterson,et al.  Sex roles as social conventions: The development of children's conceptions of sex-role stereotypes. , 1982 .

[73]  S. Gelman,et al.  The Essential Child : Origins of Essentialism in Everyday Thought , 2003 .

[74]  G. Csibra,et al.  Teleological reasoning in infancy: the naı̈ve theory of rational action , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[75]  Y. Kashima A Social Psychology of Cultural Dynamics: Examining How Cultures Are Formed, Maintained, and Transformed , 2008 .

[76]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Twelve- and 18-month-olds copy actions in terms of goals. , 2005, Developmental science.

[77]  P. Harris,et al.  Going With the Flow , 2009, Psychological science.

[78]  V. Turner,et al.  The Ritual Process. Structure and Anti-Structure , 1978 .

[79]  A. Whiten,et al.  Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens) , 2005, Animal Cognition.

[80]  Joseph Henrich,et al.  Prestige-biased cultural learning: bystander's differential attention to potential models influences children's learning , 2012 .

[81]  Pier Francesco Ferrari,et al.  Towards a bottom-up perspective on animal and human cognition , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[82]  H. Barrett,et al.  Origins of Cumulative Culture , 2012, Science.

[83]  B. Kenward Over-imitating preschoolers believe unnecessary actions are normative and enforce their performance by a third party. , 2012, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[84]  M. Carpenter,et al.  Putting the social into social learning: explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children's copying behavior. , 2012, Journal of comparative psychology.

[85]  H. Hayne,et al.  Imitation from television by 24- and 30-month-olds. , 2003 .

[86]  S. Kitayama,et al.  Handbook of Cultural Psychology , 2007 .

[87]  Marianne G. Taylor,et al.  Traditional and evaluative aspects of flexibility in gender roles, social conventions, moral rules, and physical laws. , 1995, Child development.

[88]  Frederick M. Smith,et al.  The Archetypal Actions of Ritual: A Theory of Ritual Illustrated by the Jain Rite of Worship , 1994 .

[89]  V. Turner,et al.  The Ritual Process: Structure and Anti-Structure , 1975 .

[90]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Children's coding of human action: cognitive factors influencing imitation in 3-year-olds. , 2000, Developmental science.

[91]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Young children attribute normativity to novel actions without pedagogy or normative language. , 2011, Developmental science.

[92]  D. Buss The handbook of evolutionary psychology. , 2015 .

[93]  R. Byrne,et al.  Priming primates: Human and otherwise , 1998, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[94]  H. Whitehouse Modes of Religiosity: A Cognitive Theory of Religious Transmission , 2004 .

[95]  Hannes Rakoczy,et al.  Why do children overimitate? Normativity is crucial. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[96]  E. Markman,et al.  Precision of imitation as a function of preschoolers' understanding of the goal of the demonstration. , 2006, Developmental psychology.

[97]  Cristine H. Legare,et al.  Evaluating ritual efficacy: Evidence from the supernatural , 2012, Cognition.

[98]  A. Whiten,et al.  Where culture takes hold: "overimitation" and its flexible deployment in Western, Aboriginal, and Bushmen children. , 2014, Child development.