Dynamics of the TIT FOR TAT strategy during predator inspection in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata)
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] D. E. Matthews. Evolution and the Theory of Games , 1977 .
[2] Anne E. Magurran,et al. Provenance, shoal size and the sociobiology of predator-evasion behaviour in minnow shoals , 1987, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.
[3] W. Hamilton. Geometry for the selfish herd. , 1971, Journal of theoretical biology.
[4] Anne E. Magurran,et al. Dicing with death: predator inspection behaviour in minnow shoals , 1986 .
[5] Manfred Milinski,et al. Tit for Tat: sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus) ‘trusting’ a cooperating partner , 1990 .
[6] Lee Alan Dugatkin,et al. Guppies and the TIT FOR TAT strategy: preference based on past interaction , 1991, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[7] Ronald Noë,et al. A veto game played by baboons: a challenge to the use of the Prisoner's Dilemma as a paradigm for reciprocity and cooperation , 1990, Animal Behaviour.
[8] L. Dugatkin,et al. Predator inspection, shoaling and foraging under predation hazard in the Trinidadian guppy,Poecilia reticulata , 1992, Environmental Biology of Fishes.
[9] M. Milinski,et al. Do sticklebacks cooperate repeatedly in reciprocal pairs? , 1990, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[10] J. Meyer,et al. The ontogeny of twilight migration patterns in grunts (Pisces: Haemulidae) , 1982, Animal Behaviour.
[11] W. Hamilton,et al. The Evolution of Cooperation , 1984 .
[12] M. Milinski. TIT FOR TAT in sticklebacks and the evolution of cooperation , 1987, Nature.
[13] R. Boyd. Mistakes allow evolutionary stability in the repeated prisoner's dilemma game. , 1989, Journal of theoretical biology.
[14] P. Motta. Response by potential prey to coral reef fish predators , 1983, Animal Behaviour.
[15] B. Seghers. Analysis of geographic variation in the antipredator adaptations of the guppy : Poecilia reticulata , 1973 .
[16] Anne E. Magurran,et al. Predator model recognition and response habituation in shoaling minnows , 1986, Animal Behaviour.
[17] J. F. Gilliam,et al. Feeding under predation hazard: response of the guppy and Hart's rivulus from sites with contrasting predation hazard , 1987, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[18] Anne E. Magurran,et al. The inheritance and development of minnow anti-predator behaviour , 1990, Animal Behaviour.
[19] W. Dominey,et al. Mobbing in colonially nesting fishes, especially the bluegill, Lepomis macrochirus , 1983 .
[20] A. Magurran,et al. Population differences in predator recognition and attack cone avoidance in the guppy Poecilia reticulata , 1990, Animal Behaviour.
[21] Manfred Milinski,et al. No alternative to tit-for-tat cooperation in sticklebacks , 1990, Animal Behaviour.
[22] R. Boyd,et al. Is the repeated prisoner's dilemma a good model of reciprocal altruism? , 1988 .
[23] R. Boyd,et al. No pure strategy is evolutionarily stable in the repeated Prisoner's Dilemma game , 1987, Nature.
[24] L. Dugatkin. Do guppies play TIT FOR TAT during predator inspection visits? , 1988, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[25] L. Dugatkin,et al. Tit-For-Tat in guppies (Poecilia reticulata): the relative nature of cooperation and defection during predator inspection , 1991, Evolutionary Ecology.
[26] L. Dugatkin. Predator inspection, tit-for-tat and shoaling: a comment on Masters & Waite , 1991, Animal Behaviour.
[27] N. Metcalfe,et al. Tit-for-tat cooperation in sticklebacks: a critique of Milinski , 1990, Animal Behaviour.