Implicit learning and implicit memory in moderate to severe memory disorders

Numerous experimental psychology studies have established firmly that important parts of the human cognitive process operate automatically without the conscious or explicit control of the subjects (9). Such processes can concern memorization of episodes from life in a way that will subsequently have an implicit influence on our behavior (such as decision-making or reaction time). They can equally assist acquisition of more complex knowledge from our surroundings, by the automatic capture of the statistical regularities found in them (see Chapter “Introducing implicit learning: from the laboratory to the real life”, E. Bigand and C. Delbe). This is the way, for example, that a baby learns to speak its mother tongue. Without intent, and not consciously, it extracts, progressively, different words from the flow of an adult’s speech; then it learns the rules of grammar, from which it is able to use the words in a sentence. This type of learning can be considered as a method by which the individual adapts to his or her environment (42) and, according to Reber (45), must derive, in evolutionary terms, from a very old mechanism appearing before the emergence of consciousness. This evolutionary hypothesis relies on several different properties of implicit processes.

[1]  E. D. Haan,et al.  Implicit Learning in Memory Rehabilitation: A Meta-Analysis on Errorless Learning and Vanishing Cues Methods , 2003, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[2]  Thierry Meulemans,et al.  Implicit learning of complex information in amnesia , 2003, Brain and Cognition.

[3]  Arthur S. Reber,et al.  Implicit and explicit learning: individual differences and IQ. , 1991 .

[4]  B. Knowlton,et al.  Intact implicit habit learning in Alzheimer's disease. , 2002, Behavioral neuroscience.

[5]  John D. E. Gabrieli,et al.  Intact acquisition and long-term retention of mirror-tracing skill in Alzheimer's disease and in global amnesia. , 1993 .

[6]  L. Squire,et al.  Artificial grammar learning depends on implicit acquisition of both abstract and exemplar-specific information. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[7]  M. Gluck,et al.  Probabilistic classification learning in amnesia. , 1994, Learning & memory.

[8]  J. Grafman,et al.  Implicit learning in patients with Alzheimer's disease. , 1990, Pharmacopsychiatry.

[9]  Nathalie Bier,et al.  Face–name association learning in early Alzheimer's disease: A comparison of learning methods and their underlying mechanisms , 2008, Neuropsychological rehabilitation.

[10]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Computer learning by memory-impaired patients: Acquisition and retention of complex knowledge , 1986, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  S. McAdams,et al.  Implicit learning of musical timbre sequences: statistical regularities confronted with acoustical (dis)similarities. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[12]  E. Salmon,et al.  Training early Alzheimer patients to use a mobile phone. , 2002, Acta neurologica Belgica.

[13]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Implicit learning: news from the front , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[14]  D. Willingham,et al.  Patients with Alzheimer's disease who cannot perform some motor skills show normal learning of other motor skills. , 1997, Neuropsychology.

[15]  Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al.  Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults , 1999, Cognition.

[16]  T. Hopper "They're just going to get worse anyway": perspectives on rehabilitation for nursing home residents with dementia. , 2003, Journal of communication disorders.

[17]  Ilona Roth,et al.  Relearning face-name associations in early Alzheimer's disease. , 2002, Neuropsychology.

[18]  E. Salmon,et al.  Implicit/explicit memory dissociation in Alzheimer's disease: the consequence of inappropriate processing? , 2008, Neuropsychology.

[19]  A. Halpern,et al.  Implicit Memory for Music in Alzheimer's Disease , 2000 .

[20]  S. Samson,et al.  [Pr\é\servation des apprentissages implicites en musique dans le vieillissement normal et la maladie d'Alzheimer] , 2008 .

[21]  M. Linden,et al.  Cognitive Intervention Case Studies: Putting Names on Faces: Use of a Spaced Retrieval Strategy in a Patient With Dementia of the Alzheimer Type , 1998 .

[22]  Seth J. Ramus,et al.  Intact Artificial Grammar Learning in Amnesia: Dissociation of Classification Learning and Explicit Memory for Specific Instances , 1992 .

[23]  D. Balota,et al.  Implicit Memory and the Formation of New Associations in Nondemented Parkinson′s Disease Individuals and Individuals with Senile Dementia of the Alzheimer Type: A Serial Reaction Time (SRT) Investigation , 1993, Brain and Cognition.

[24]  B. Milner Preface: Material-specific and generalized memory loss , 1968 .

[25]  David A. Balota,et al.  Semantic priming effects, lexical repetition effects, and contextual disambiguation effects in healthy aged individuals and individuals with senile dementia of the Alzheimer type , 1991, Brain and Language.

[26]  M. Nissen,et al.  Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[27]  A. Reber Implicit learning and tacit knowledge , 1993 .

[28]  L. Squire Mechanisms of memory. , 1986, Lancet.

[29]  Brenda Milner,et al.  The medial temporal-lobe amnesic syndrome. , 2005, The Psychiatric clinics of North America.

[30]  Gabriele Ende,et al.  MR spectroscopy in Alzheimer’s disease: gender differences in probabilistic learning capacity , 2003, Neurobiology of Aging.

[31]  M. Nissen,et al.  Implicit learning in patients with probable Alzheimer's disease , 1987, Neurology.

[32]  L. Squire,et al.  Parallel brain systems for learning with and without awareness. , 1994, Learning & memory.

[33]  E. Winograd,et al.  The mere exposure effect in patients with Alzheimer's disease. , 1999, Neuropsychology.

[34]  N. Hunkin,et al.  UNLEARNING JOHN MAJOR: THE USE OF ERRORLESS LEARNING IN THE REACQUISITION OF PROPER NAMES FOLLOWING HERPES SIMPLEX ENCEPHALITIS. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[35]  M. Linden,et al.  Normal Mere Exposure Effect with Impaired Recognition in Alzheimer's Disease , 2002, Cortex.

[36]  Barbara J Knowlton,et al.  Analysis of probabilistic classification learning in patients with Parkinson's disease before and after pallidotomy surgery. , 2003, Learning & memory.

[37]  Muriel Vandenberghe,et al.  Can amnesic patients learn without awareness? New evidence comparing deterministic and probabilistic sequence learning , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  D. Andrewes The Work Rehabilitation of a Herpes Simplex Encephalitis Patient with Anterograde Amnesia , 1999 .

[39]  J. Gabrieli,et al.  Word-stem completion priming in healthy aging and Alzheimer's disease: the effects of age, cognitive status, and encoding. , 1999, Neuropsychology.

[40]  C. P. Hughes,et al.  A New Clinical Scale for the Staging of Dementia , 1982, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[41]  M. Merello,et al.  Explicit and implicit learning in patients with Alzheimer disease and Parkinson disease with dementia. , 1999, Neuropsychiatry, neuropsychology, and behavioral neurology.

[42]  E. Glisky Acquisition and transfer of declarative and procedural knowledge by memory-impaired patients: A computer data-entry task , 1992, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Can patients with Alzheimer's disease learn a category implicitly? , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  L. Squire,et al.  Learning about categories in the absence of memory. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[45]  P. Reber,et al.  Artificial grammar learning in Alzheimer’s disease , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[46]  Annie Vinter,et al.  Feature creation as a byproduct of attentional processing , 1998 .

[47]  L. Squire,et al.  The Neuropsychology of Memory , 1990 .

[48]  R N Aslin,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996, Science.

[49]  Wouter Hulstijn,et al.  Motor-Skill Learning in Alzheimer’s Disease: A Review with an Eye to the Clinical Practice , 2007, Neuropsychology Review.

[50]  L. Squire,et al.  The learning of categories: parallel brain systems for item memory and category knowledge. , 1993, Science.

[51]  L R Squire,et al.  Learning about categories that are defined by object-like stimuli despite impaired declarative memory. , 1999, Behavioral neuroscience.

[52]  L. Berg Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR). , 1988, Psychopharmacology bulletin.

[53]  I. Peretz,et al.  Implicit and Explicit Emotional Memory for Melodies in Alzheimer's Disease and Depression , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[54]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  The case of K.C.: contributions of a memory-impaired person to memory theory , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[55]  János Kálmán,et al.  Are Alzheimer's disease patients able to learn visual prototypes? , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[56]  J. H. Howard,et al.  Adult age differences in the rate of learning serial patterns: evidence from direct and indirect tests. , 1992, Psychology and aging.

[57]  E. Newport,et al.  WORD SEGMENTATION : THE ROLE OF DISTRIBUTIONAL CUES , 1996 .

[58]  A. Reber The cognitive unconscious: An evolutionary perspective , 1992, Consciousness and Cognition.